• Search Menu

Sign in through your institution

  • Browse content in A - General Economics and Teaching
  • Browse content in A1 - General Economics
  • A12 - Relation of Economics to Other Disciplines
  • A14 - Sociology of Economics
  • Browse content in B - History of Economic Thought, Methodology, and Heterodox Approaches
  • Browse content in B4 - Economic Methodology
  • B41 - Economic Methodology
  • Browse content in C - Mathematical and Quantitative Methods
  • Browse content in C1 - Econometric and Statistical Methods and Methodology: General
  • C18 - Methodological Issues: General
  • Browse content in C2 - Single Equation Models; Single Variables
  • C21 - Cross-Sectional Models; Spatial Models; Treatment Effect Models; Quantile Regressions
  • Browse content in C3 - Multiple or Simultaneous Equation Models; Multiple Variables
  • C38 - Classification Methods; Cluster Analysis; Principal Components; Factor Models
  • Browse content in C5 - Econometric Modeling
  • C59 - Other
  • Browse content in C8 - Data Collection and Data Estimation Methodology; Computer Programs
  • C80 - General
  • C81 - Methodology for Collecting, Estimating, and Organizing Microeconomic Data; Data Access
  • C83 - Survey Methods; Sampling Methods
  • Browse content in C9 - Design of Experiments
  • C93 - Field Experiments
  • Browse content in D - Microeconomics
  • Browse content in D0 - General
  • D02 - Institutions: Design, Formation, Operations, and Impact
  • D03 - Behavioral Microeconomics: Underlying Principles
  • D04 - Microeconomic Policy: Formulation; Implementation, and Evaluation
  • Browse content in D1 - Household Behavior and Family Economics
  • D10 - General
  • D12 - Consumer Economics: Empirical Analysis
  • D14 - Household Saving; Personal Finance
  • Browse content in D2 - Production and Organizations
  • D22 - Firm Behavior: Empirical Analysis
  • D24 - Production; Cost; Capital; Capital, Total Factor, and Multifactor Productivity; Capacity
  • Browse content in D3 - Distribution
  • D31 - Personal Income, Wealth, and Their Distributions
  • Browse content in D6 - Welfare Economics
  • D61 - Allocative Efficiency; Cost-Benefit Analysis
  • D62 - Externalities
  • D63 - Equity, Justice, Inequality, and Other Normative Criteria and Measurement
  • Browse content in D7 - Analysis of Collective Decision-Making
  • D72 - Political Processes: Rent-seeking, Lobbying, Elections, Legislatures, and Voting Behavior
  • D73 - Bureaucracy; Administrative Processes in Public Organizations; Corruption
  • D74 - Conflict; Conflict Resolution; Alliances; Revolutions
  • Browse content in D8 - Information, Knowledge, and Uncertainty
  • D83 - Search; Learning; Information and Knowledge; Communication; Belief; Unawareness
  • D85 - Network Formation and Analysis: Theory
  • D86 - Economics of Contract: Theory
  • Browse content in D9 - Micro-Based Behavioral Economics
  • D91 - Role and Effects of Psychological, Emotional, Social, and Cognitive Factors on Decision Making
  • D92 - Intertemporal Firm Choice, Investment, Capacity, and Financing
  • Browse content in E - Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Browse content in E2 - Consumption, Saving, Production, Investment, Labor Markets, and Informal Economy
  • E23 - Production
  • E24 - Employment; Unemployment; Wages; Intergenerational Income Distribution; Aggregate Human Capital; Aggregate Labor Productivity
  • Browse content in E4 - Money and Interest Rates
  • E42 - Monetary Systems; Standards; Regimes; Government and the Monetary System; Payment Systems
  • Browse content in E5 - Monetary Policy, Central Banking, and the Supply of Money and Credit
  • E52 - Monetary Policy
  • E58 - Central Banks and Their Policies
  • Browse content in E6 - Macroeconomic Policy, Macroeconomic Aspects of Public Finance, and General Outlook
  • E60 - General
  • E61 - Policy Objectives; Policy Designs and Consistency; Policy Coordination
  • E62 - Fiscal Policy
  • E65 - Studies of Particular Policy Episodes
  • Browse content in F - International Economics
  • Browse content in F0 - General
  • F01 - Global Outlook
  • Browse content in F1 - Trade
  • F10 - General
  • F11 - Neoclassical Models of Trade
  • F13 - Trade Policy; International Trade Organizations
  • F14 - Empirical Studies of Trade
  • F15 - Economic Integration
  • F19 - Other
  • Browse content in F2 - International Factor Movements and International Business
  • F21 - International Investment; Long-Term Capital Movements
  • F22 - International Migration
  • F23 - Multinational Firms; International Business
  • Browse content in F3 - International Finance
  • F32 - Current Account Adjustment; Short-Term Capital Movements
  • F34 - International Lending and Debt Problems
  • F35 - Foreign Aid
  • F36 - Financial Aspects of Economic Integration
  • Browse content in F4 - Macroeconomic Aspects of International Trade and Finance
  • F41 - Open Economy Macroeconomics
  • F42 - International Policy Coordination and Transmission
  • F43 - Economic Growth of Open Economies
  • Browse content in F5 - International Relations, National Security, and International Political Economy
  • F50 - General
  • F52 - National Security; Economic Nationalism
  • F53 - International Agreements and Observance; International Organizations
  • F55 - International Institutional Arrangements
  • Browse content in F6 - Economic Impacts of Globalization
  • F61 - Microeconomic Impacts
  • F63 - Economic Development
  • F66 - Labor
  • Browse content in G - Financial Economics
  • Browse content in G0 - General
  • G01 - Financial Crises
  • Browse content in G1 - General Financial Markets
  • G10 - General
  • G15 - International Financial Markets
  • G18 - Government Policy and Regulation
  • Browse content in G2 - Financial Institutions and Services
  • G20 - General
  • G21 - Banks; Depository Institutions; Micro Finance Institutions; Mortgages
  • G22 - Insurance; Insurance Companies; Actuarial Studies
  • G23 - Non-bank Financial Institutions; Financial Instruments; Institutional Investors
  • G28 - Government Policy and Regulation
  • Browse content in G3 - Corporate Finance and Governance
  • G32 - Financing Policy; Financial Risk and Risk Management; Capital and Ownership Structure; Value of Firms; Goodwill
  • G33 - Bankruptcy; Liquidation
  • G38 - Government Policy and Regulation
  • Browse content in H - Public Economics
  • Browse content in H1 - Structure and Scope of Government
  • H11 - Structure, Scope, and Performance of Government
  • Browse content in H2 - Taxation, Subsidies, and Revenue
  • H20 - General
  • H23 - Externalities; Redistributive Effects; Environmental Taxes and Subsidies
  • H25 - Business Taxes and Subsidies
  • H26 - Tax Evasion and Avoidance
  • H27 - Other Sources of Revenue
  • Browse content in H3 - Fiscal Policies and Behavior of Economic Agents
  • H31 - Household
  • Browse content in H4 - Publicly Provided Goods
  • H41 - Public Goods
  • H43 - Project Evaluation; Social Discount Rate
  • Browse content in H5 - National Government Expenditures and Related Policies
  • H52 - Government Expenditures and Education
  • H53 - Government Expenditures and Welfare Programs
  • H54 - Infrastructures; Other Public Investment and Capital Stock
  • H55 - Social Security and Public Pensions
  • H56 - National Security and War
  • H57 - Procurement
  • Browse content in H6 - National Budget, Deficit, and Debt
  • H60 - General
  • H61 - Budget; Budget Systems
  • Browse content in H7 - State and Local Government; Intergovernmental Relations
  • H71 - State and Local Taxation, Subsidies, and Revenue
  • H75 - State and Local Government: Health; Education; Welfare; Public Pensions
  • H77 - Intergovernmental Relations; Federalism; Secession
  • Browse content in H8 - Miscellaneous Issues
  • H83 - Public Administration; Public Sector Accounting and Audits
  • H84 - Disaster Aid
  • Browse content in I - Health, Education, and Welfare
  • Browse content in I0 - General
  • I00 - General
  • Browse content in I1 - Health
  • I10 - General
  • I12 - Health Behavior
  • I15 - Health and Economic Development
  • I18 - Government Policy; Regulation; Public Health
  • Browse content in I2 - Education and Research Institutions
  • I20 - General
  • I21 - Analysis of Education
  • I22 - Educational Finance; Financial Aid
  • I24 - Education and Inequality
  • I25 - Education and Economic Development
  • I28 - Government Policy
  • Browse content in I3 - Welfare, Well-Being, and Poverty
  • I30 - General
  • I31 - General Welfare
  • I32 - Measurement and Analysis of Poverty
  • I38 - Government Policy; Provision and Effects of Welfare Programs
  • Browse content in J - Labor and Demographic Economics
  • Browse content in J0 - General
  • J01 - Labor Economics: General
  • J08 - Labor Economics Policies
  • Browse content in J1 - Demographic Economics
  • J10 - General
  • J11 - Demographic Trends, Macroeconomic Effects, and Forecasts
  • J12 - Marriage; Marital Dissolution; Family Structure; Domestic Abuse
  • J13 - Fertility; Family Planning; Child Care; Children; Youth
  • J15 - Economics of Minorities, Races, Indigenous Peoples, and Immigrants; Non-labor Discrimination
  • J16 - Economics of Gender; Non-labor Discrimination
  • J17 - Value of Life; Forgone Income
  • J18 - Public Policy
  • Browse content in J2 - Demand and Supply of Labor
  • J21 - Labor Force and Employment, Size, and Structure
  • J22 - Time Allocation and Labor Supply
  • J23 - Labor Demand
  • J24 - Human Capital; Skills; Occupational Choice; Labor Productivity
  • J26 - Retirement; Retirement Policies
  • J28 - Safety; Job Satisfaction; Related Public Policy
  • Browse content in J3 - Wages, Compensation, and Labor Costs
  • J38 - Public Policy
  • Browse content in J4 - Particular Labor Markets
  • J48 - Public Policy
  • Browse content in J5 - Labor-Management Relations, Trade Unions, and Collective Bargaining
  • J58 - Public Policy
  • Browse content in J6 - Mobility, Unemployment, Vacancies, and Immigrant Workers
  • J61 - Geographic Labor Mobility; Immigrant Workers
  • J62 - Job, Occupational, and Intergenerational Mobility
  • J63 - Turnover; Vacancies; Layoffs
  • J68 - Public Policy
  • Browse content in J8 - Labor Standards: National and International
  • J88 - Public Policy
  • Browse content in K - Law and Economics
  • Browse content in K2 - Regulation and Business Law
  • K23 - Regulated Industries and Administrative Law
  • Browse content in K3 - Other Substantive Areas of Law
  • K32 - Environmental, Health, and Safety Law
  • K34 - Tax Law
  • Browse content in K4 - Legal Procedure, the Legal System, and Illegal Behavior
  • K40 - General
  • K41 - Litigation Process
  • K42 - Illegal Behavior and the Enforcement of Law
  • Browse content in L - Industrial Organization
  • Browse content in L1 - Market Structure, Firm Strategy, and Market Performance
  • L11 - Production, Pricing, and Market Structure; Size Distribution of Firms
  • L14 - Transactional Relationships; Contracts and Reputation; Networks
  • L16 - Industrial Organization and Macroeconomics: Industrial Structure and Structural Change; Industrial Price Indices
  • Browse content in L2 - Firm Objectives, Organization, and Behavior
  • L20 - General
  • L23 - Organization of Production
  • L25 - Firm Performance: Size, Diversification, and Scope
  • L26 - Entrepreneurship
  • Browse content in L3 - Nonprofit Organizations and Public Enterprise
  • L33 - Comparison of Public and Private Enterprises and Nonprofit Institutions; Privatization; Contracting Out
  • Browse content in L5 - Regulation and Industrial Policy
  • L51 - Economics of Regulation
  • L52 - Industrial Policy; Sectoral Planning Methods
  • Browse content in L9 - Industry Studies: Transportation and Utilities
  • L94 - Electric Utilities
  • L97 - Utilities: General
  • L98 - Government Policy
  • Browse content in M - Business Administration and Business Economics; Marketing; Accounting; Personnel Economics
  • Browse content in M5 - Personnel Economics
  • M53 - Training
  • Browse content in N - Economic History
  • Browse content in N1 - Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics; Industrial Structure; Growth; Fluctuations
  • N10 - General, International, or Comparative
  • Browse content in N3 - Labor and Consumers, Demography, Education, Health, Welfare, Income, Wealth, Religion, and Philanthropy
  • N35 - Asia including Middle East
  • Browse content in N5 - Agriculture, Natural Resources, Environment, and Extractive Industries
  • N55 - Asia including Middle East
  • N57 - Africa; Oceania
  • Browse content in N7 - Transport, Trade, Energy, Technology, and Other Services
  • N77 - Africa; Oceania
  • Browse content in O - Economic Development, Innovation, Technological Change, and Growth
  • Browse content in O1 - Economic Development
  • O10 - General
  • O11 - Macroeconomic Analyses of Economic Development
  • O12 - Microeconomic Analyses of Economic Development
  • O13 - Agriculture; Natural Resources; Energy; Environment; Other Primary Products
  • O14 - Industrialization; Manufacturing and Service Industries; Choice of Technology
  • O15 - Human Resources; Human Development; Income Distribution; Migration
  • O16 - Financial Markets; Saving and Capital Investment; Corporate Finance and Governance
  • O17 - Formal and Informal Sectors; Shadow Economy; Institutional Arrangements
  • O18 - Urban, Rural, Regional, and Transportation Analysis; Housing; Infrastructure
  • O19 - International Linkages to Development; Role of International Organizations
  • Browse content in O2 - Development Planning and Policy
  • O20 - General
  • O22 - Project Analysis
  • O23 - Fiscal and Monetary Policy in Development
  • O24 - Trade Policy; Factor Movement Policy; Foreign Exchange Policy
  • O25 - Industrial Policy
  • Browse content in O3 - Innovation; Research and Development; Technological Change; Intellectual Property Rights
  • O31 - Innovation and Invention: Processes and Incentives
  • O32 - Management of Technological Innovation and R&D
  • O33 - Technological Change: Choices and Consequences; Diffusion Processes
  • O34 - Intellectual Property and Intellectual Capital
  • O38 - Government Policy
  • Browse content in O4 - Economic Growth and Aggregate Productivity
  • O40 - General
  • O41 - One, Two, and Multisector Growth Models
  • O43 - Institutions and Growth
  • O44 - Environment and Growth
  • O47 - Empirical Studies of Economic Growth; Aggregate Productivity; Cross-Country Output Convergence
  • Browse content in O5 - Economywide Country Studies
  • O55 - Africa
  • O57 - Comparative Studies of Countries
  • Browse content in P - Economic Systems
  • Browse content in P1 - Capitalist Systems
  • P14 - Property Rights
  • Browse content in P2 - Socialist Systems and Transitional Economies
  • P26 - Political Economy; Property Rights
  • Browse content in P3 - Socialist Institutions and Their Transitions
  • P30 - General
  • Browse content in P4 - Other Economic Systems
  • P43 - Public Economics; Financial Economics
  • P46 - Consumer Economics; Health; Education and Training; Welfare, Income, Wealth, and Poverty
  • P48 - Political Economy; Legal Institutions; Property Rights; Natural Resources; Energy; Environment; Regional Studies
  • Browse content in Q - Agricultural and Natural Resource Economics; Environmental and Ecological Economics
  • Browse content in Q0 - General
  • Q01 - Sustainable Development
  • Browse content in Q1 - Agriculture
  • Q10 - General
  • Q12 - Micro Analysis of Farm Firms, Farm Households, and Farm Input Markets
  • Q13 - Agricultural Markets and Marketing; Cooperatives; Agribusiness
  • Q14 - Agricultural Finance
  • Q15 - Land Ownership and Tenure; Land Reform; Land Use; Irrigation; Agriculture and Environment
  • Q16 - R&D; Agricultural Technology; Biofuels; Agricultural Extension Services
  • Q17 - Agriculture in International Trade
  • Q18 - Agricultural Policy; Food Policy
  • Browse content in Q2 - Renewable Resources and Conservation
  • Q25 - Water
  • Browse content in Q3 - Nonrenewable Resources and Conservation
  • Q30 - General
  • Q33 - Resource Booms
  • Browse content in Q4 - Energy
  • Q41 - Demand and Supply; Prices
  • Q43 - Energy and the Macroeconomy
  • Browse content in Q5 - Environmental Economics
  • Q51 - Valuation of Environmental Effects
  • Q52 - Pollution Control Adoption Costs; Distributional Effects; Employment Effects
  • Q54 - Climate; Natural Disasters; Global Warming
  • Q56 - Environment and Development; Environment and Trade; Sustainability; Environmental Accounts and Accounting; Environmental Equity; Population Growth
  • Q57 - Ecological Economics: Ecosystem Services; Biodiversity Conservation; Bioeconomics; Industrial Ecology
  • Q58 - Government Policy
  • Browse content in R - Urban, Rural, Regional, Real Estate, and Transportation Economics
  • Browse content in R1 - General Regional Economics
  • R11 - Regional Economic Activity: Growth, Development, Environmental Issues, and Changes
  • R12 - Size and Spatial Distributions of Regional Economic Activity
  • R13 - General Equilibrium and Welfare Economic Analysis of Regional Economies
  • R14 - Land Use Patterns
  • Browse content in R2 - Household Analysis
  • R20 - General
  • R23 - Regional Migration; Regional Labor Markets; Population; Neighborhood Characteristics
  • R28 - Government Policy
  • Browse content in R3 - Real Estate Markets, Spatial Production Analysis, and Firm Location
  • R38 - Government Policy
  • Browse content in R4 - Transportation Economics
  • R40 - General
  • R41 - Transportation: Demand, Supply, and Congestion; Travel Time; Safety and Accidents; Transportation Noise
  • R48 - Government Pricing and Policy
  • Browse content in R5 - Regional Government Analysis
  • R52 - Land Use and Other Regulations
  • Browse content in Y - Miscellaneous Categories
  • Y8 - Related Disciplines
  • Browse content in Z - Other Special Topics
  • Browse content in Z1 - Cultural Economics; Economic Sociology; Economic Anthropology
  • Z13 - Economic Sociology; Economic Anthropology; Social and Economic Stratification
  • Advance articles
  • Author Guidelines
  • Open Access
  • About The World Bank Research Observer
  • About the World Bank
  • Editorial Board
  • Advertising and Corporate Services
  • Self-Archiving Policy
  • Dispatch Dates
  • Journals on Oxford Academic
  • Books on Oxford Academic

World Bank

  • < Previous

Child Development in a Changing World: Risks and Opportunities

  • Article contents
  • Figures & tables
  • Supplementary Data

Jo Boyden, Stefan Dercon, Abhijeet Singh, Child Development in a Changing World: Risks and Opportunities, The World Bank Research Observer , Volume 30, Issue 2, August 2015, Pages 193–219, https://doi.org/10.1093/wbro/lku009

  • Permissions Icon Permissions

This review explores current understandings of child development and the consequences for children of risk exposure in low- and middle-income countries by integrating empirical evidence from development economics with insights from allied social science disciplines. It provides a holistic perspective that highlights the synergies between children's developmental domains, drawing particular attention to dimensions such as self-efficacy, self-esteem and aspirations, which have had only limited treatment in the economics literature to date, especially in developing countries. It concludes that there is strong evidence of dynamic relationships between risk factors in early childhood and later outcomes across multiple developmental domains, emphasizing the heightened effect of shocks to the care environment and the cumulative effect of multiple shocks. It also concludes that risk is distributed unevenly, with children who are both in poverty and disadvantaged socially according to, for example, their ethnicity bearing the greatest burden; within a household, gender, birth order and other factors mean that some suffer disproportionately from shortfalls and incomplete protection. However, this review finds that low endowments in early childhood can be at least partially compensated for through improved environments and investments in later childhood, emphasizing the resilience of some children. The review goes on to explore the impact on children of dramatic socio-economic changes that have occurred in recent years with rapid growth across most developing countries. It highlights four key forces for change—fall in absolute poverty, increased access to services, changing household incentives for investing in children, and changing social and cultural values—and stresses the ambiguous effects on the welfare of children and their long-term prospects. In so doing, the review aims to consolidate emerging evidence on how risks and opportunities for child development may have changed in these dynamic contexts.

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Short-term Access

To purchase short-term access, please sign in to your personal account above.

Don't already have a personal account? Register

Month: Total Views:
November 2016 1
December 2016 5
January 2017 12
February 2017 39
March 2017 41
April 2017 34
May 2017 32
June 2017 18
July 2017 10
August 2017 15
September 2017 15
October 2017 8
November 2017 9
December 2017 13
January 2018 6
February 2018 17
March 2018 22
April 2018 29
May 2018 20
June 2018 20
July 2018 8
August 2018 15
September 2018 9
October 2018 5
November 2018 11
December 2018 6
January 2019 1
February 2019 8
March 2019 1
April 2019 13
May 2019 17
July 2019 13
August 2019 9
September 2019 9
October 2019 11
November 2019 3
December 2019 6
January 2020 1
February 2020 10
March 2020 10
April 2020 3
May 2020 7
June 2020 5
July 2020 1
August 2020 3
September 2020 14
October 2020 23
November 2020 10
December 2020 13
January 2021 6
February 2021 6
March 2021 4
April 2021 6
May 2021 2
July 2021 4
August 2021 4
September 2021 2
October 2021 5
November 2021 3
December 2021 1
January 2022 3
February 2022 11
March 2022 9
April 2022 18
May 2022 5
June 2022 7
July 2022 4
August 2022 2
September 2022 2
October 2022 2
November 2022 6
December 2022 4
January 2023 8
March 2023 2
April 2023 1
May 2023 1
July 2023 2
August 2023 2
September 2023 7
October 2023 9
November 2023 11
December 2023 4
January 2024 15
February 2024 5
March 2024 2
April 2024 14
June 2024 3
July 2024 8
August 2024 1

Email alerts

Citing articles via.

  • Recommend to your Library

Affiliations

  • Online ISSN 1564-6971
  • Print ISSN 0257-3032
  • Copyright © 2024 World Bank
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Warning: The NCBI web site requires JavaScript to function. more...

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Committee on the Science of Children Birth to Age 8: Deepening and Broadening the Foundation for Success; Board on Children, Youth, and Families; Institute of Medicine; National Research Council; Allen LR, Kelly BB, editors. Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8: A Unifying Foundation. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2015 Jul 23.

Cover of Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8

Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8: A Unifying Foundation.

  • Hardcopy Version at National Academies Press

4 Child Development and Early Learning

The domains of child development and early learning are discussed in different terms and categorized in different ways in the various fields and disciplines that are involved in research, practice, and policy related to children from birth through age 8. To organize the discussion in this report, the committee elected to use the approach and overarching terms depicted in Figure 4-1 . The committee does not intend to present this as a single best set of terms or a single best categorical organization. Indeed, it is essential to recognize that the domains shown in Figure 4-1 are not easily separable and that a case can be made for multiple different categorizations. For example, different disciplines and researchers have categorized different general cognitive processes under the categorical term “executive function.” General cognitive processes also relate to learning competencies such as persistence and engagement. Similarly, self-regulation has both cognitive and emotional dimensions. It is sometimes categorized as a part of executive function, as a part of socioemotional competence, or as a part of learning competencies. Attention and memory could be considered a part of general cognitive processes, as embedded within executive function, or linked to learning competencies related to persistence. Mental health is closely linked to socioemotional competence, but is also inseparable from health.

Report's organizational approach for the domains of child development and early learning.

The challenge of cleanly separating these concepts highlights a key attribute of all of these domains, which is that they do not develop or operate in isolation. Each enables and mutually supports learning and development in the others. Therefore, the importance of the interactions among the domains is emphasized throughout this chapter. For example, socioemotional competence is important for self-regulation, as are certain cognitive skills, and both emotional and cognitive self-regulation are important for children to be able to exercise learning competencies. Similarly, although certain skills and concept knowledge are distinct to developing proficiency in particular subject areas, learning in these subject areas also both requires and supports general cognitive skills such as reasoning and attention, as well as learning competencies and socioemotional competence. In an overarching example of interactions, a child's security both physically and in relationships creates the context in which learning is most achievable across all of the domains.

It is less important that all fields of research, practice, and policy adhere to the exact same categorizations, and more important that all conduct their work in a way that is cognizant and inclusive of all the elements that contribute to child development and early learning, and that all fields recognize that they are interactive and mutually reinforcing rather than hierarchical. This point foreshadows a theme that is addressed more fully in subsequent chapters. Because different fields and sectors may not use the same categorizations and vocabulary for these domains and skills, developing practices and policies that support more consistent and continuous development and early learning across birth through age 8 will require a concerted effort to communicate clearly and come to a mutual understanding of the goals for children. To communicate across fields and between research and practice communities requires being aware of the different categorical frameworks and terms that are used and being able to discuss the various concepts and content—and their implications—with clarity across those different frameworks. Practitioners and policy makers will be aided in achieving greater precision and clarity in their actions and decisions if those conducting and communicating future research keep this challenge in mind across domains, especially in those cases in which the taxonomy is most variable (e.g., self-regulation, executive function, general learning competencies).

With these caveats in mind, the remainder of this chapter addresses in turn the domains of child development and early learning depicted in Figure 4-1 : cognitive development, including learning of specific subjects; general learning competencies; socioemotional development; and physical development and health. The final section examines a key overarching issue: the effects on child development and early learning of the stress and adversity that is also an important theme in the discussion of the interaction between biology and environment in Chapter 3 .

  • COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

This section highlights what is known about cognitive development in young children. It begins with key concepts from research viewpoints that have contributed to recent advances in understanding of the developing mind, and then presents the implications of this knowledge for early care and education settings. The following section addresses the learning of specific subjects, with a focus on language and mathematics.

Studies of early cognitive development have led researchers to understand the developing mind as astonishingly competent, active, and insightful from a very early age. For example, infants engage in an intuitive analysis of the statistical regularities in the speech sounds they hear en route to constructing language ( Saffran, 2003 ). Infants and toddlers derive implicit theories to explain the actions of objects and the behavior of people; these theories form the foundation for causal learning and more sophisticated understanding of the physical and social worlds. Infants and young children also are keenly responsive to what they can learn from the actions and words directed to them by other people. This capacity for joint attention may be the foundation that enables humans to benefit from culturally transmitted knowledge ( Tomasello et al., 2005 ). Infants respond to cues conveying the communicative intentions of an adult (such as eye contact and infant-directed speech) and tune in to what the adult is referring to and what can be learned about it. This “natural pedagogy” ( Csibra, 2010 ; Csibra and Gergely, 2009 ) becomes more sophisticated in the sensitivity of preschoolers to implicit pedagogical guides in adult speech directed to them ( Butler and Markman, 2012a , b , 2014 ). Young children rely so much on what they learn from others that they become astute, by the preschool years, in distinguishing adult speakers who are likely to provide them with reliable information from those who are not ( Harris, 2012 ; Jaswal, 2010 ; Koenig and Doebel, 2013 ). This connection of relationships and social interactions to cognitive development is consistent with how the brain develops and how the mind grows, and is a theme throughout this chapter.

Much of what current research shows is going on in young children's minds is not transparent in their behavior. Infants and young children may not show what they know because of competing demands on their attention, limitations in what they can do, and immature self-regulation. This is one of the reasons why developmental scientists use carefully designed experiments for elucidating what young children know and understand about the world. By designing research procedures that eliminate competing distractions and rely on simple responses (such as looking time and expressions of surprise), researchers seek to uncover cognitive processes that might otherwise be more difficult to see. Evidence derived in this experimental manner, such as the examples in the sections that follow, can be helpful in explaining young children's rapid growth in language learning, imitation, problem solving, and other skills.

Implicit Theories

One of the most important discoveries about the developing mind is how early and significantly very young children, even starting in infancy, are uniting disparate observations or discrete facts into coherent conceptual systems ( Carey, 2009 ; Gopnik and Wellman, 2012 ; Spelke and Kinzler, 2007 ). From very early on, children are not simply passive observers, registering the superficial appearance of things. Rather, they are building explanatory systems—implicit theories—that organize their knowledge. Such implicit theories contain causal principles and causal relations; these theories enable children to predict, explain, and reason about relevant phenomena and, in some cases, intervene to change them. As early as the first year of life, babies are developing incipient theories about how the world of people, other living things, objects, and numbers operates. It is important to point out that these foundational theories are not simply isolated forms of knowledge, but play a profound role in children's everyday lives and subsequent education.

One major example of an implicit theory that is already developing as early as infancy is “theory of mind,” which refers to the conceptual framework people use to reason about the mental lives of others as well as themselves. This example is discussed in detail below. Some additional illustrative examples of the development of implicit theories are provided in Box 4-1 .

Examples of the Development of Implicit Theories. Even babies hold some fundamental principles about how objects move about in space and time (Baillargeon et al., 2009). For example, babies are surprised (as measured by their increased looking time) if (more...)

Theory of Mind

People intuitively understand others' actions as motivated by desires, goals, feelings, intentions, thoughts, and other mental states, and we understand how these mental states affect one another (for example, an unfulfilled desire can evoke negative feelings and a motivation to continue trying to achieve the goal). One remarkable discovery of research on young children is that they are developing their own intuitive “map” of mental processes like these from very early in life ( Baillargeon et al., 2010 ; Saxe, 2013 ; Wellman and Woolley, 1990 ). Children's developing theory of mind transforms how they respond to people and what they learn from them. Infants and young children are beginning to understand what goes on in people's minds, and how others' feelings and thoughts are similar to and different from their own.

Infants first have a relatively simple theory of mind. They are aware of some basic characteristics: what people are looking at is a sign of what they are paying attention to; people act intentionally and are goal directed; people have positive and negative feelings in response to things around them; and people have different perceptions, goals, and feelings. Children add to this mental map as their awareness grows. From infancy on, developing theory of mind permeates everyday social interactions—affecting what and how children learn, how they react to and interact with other people, how they assess the fairness of an action, and how they evaluate themselves.

One-year-olds, for example, will look in their mother's direction when faced with someone or something unfamiliar to “read” mother's expression and determine whether this is a dangerous or benign unfamiliarity. Infants also detect when an adult makes eye contact, speaks in an infant-directed manner (such as using higher pitch and melodic intonations), and responds contingently to the infant's behavior. Under these circumstances, infants are especially attentive to what the adult says and does, thus devoting special attention to social situations in which the adult's intentions are likely to represent learning opportunities.

Other examples also illustrate how a developing theory of mind underlies children's emerging understanding of the intentions of others. Take imitation, for example. It is well established that babies and young children imitate the actions of others. Children as young as 14 to 18 months are often imitating not the literal observed action but the action they thought the actor intended—the goal or the rationale behind the action ( Gergely et al., 2002 ; Meltzoff, 1995 ). Word learning is another example in which babies' reasoning based on theory of mind plays a crucial role. By at least 15 months old, when babies hear an adult label an object, they take the speaker's intent into account by checking the speaker's focus of attention and deciding whether they think the adult indicated the object intentionally. Only when babies have evidence that the speaker intended to refer to a particular object with a label will they learn that word ( Baldwin, 1991 ; Baldwin and Moses, 2001 ; Baldwin and Tomasello, 1998 ).

Babies also can perceive the unfulfilled goals of others and intervene to help them; this is called “shared intentionality.” Babies as young as 14 months old who witness an adult struggling to reach for an object will interrupt their play to crawl over and hand the object to the adult ( Warneken and Tomasello, 2007 ). By the time they are 18 months old, shared intentionality enables toddlers to act helpfully in a variety of situations; for example, they pick up dropped objects for adults who indicate that they need assistance (but not for adults who dropped the object intentionally) ( Warneken and Tomasello, 2006 ). Developing an understanding of others' goals and preferences and how to facilitate them affects how young children interpret the behavior of people they observe and provides a basis for developing a sense of helpful versus undesirable human activity that is a foundation for later development of moral understanding (cf. Bloom, 2013 ; Hamlin et al., 2007 ; Thompson, 2012 , 2015 ).

Developing Implicit Theories: Implications for Adults

The research on the development of implicit theories in children has important implications for how adults work with and educate young children. Failure to recognize the extent to which they are construing information in terms of their lay theories can result in educational strategies that oversimplify material for children. Educational materials guided by the assumption that young children are “concrete” thinkers—that they cannot deal with abstraction or reason hypothetically—leads educators to focus on simple, descriptive activities that can deprive children of opportunities to advance their conceptual frameworks. Designing effective materials in a given domain or subject matter requires knowing what implicit theories children hold, what core causal principles they use, and what misconceptions and gaps in knowledge they have, and then using empirically validated steps to help lead them to a more accurate, more advanced conceptual framework.

Statistical Learning

Statistical learning refers to the range of ways in which children, even babies, are implicitly sensitive to the statistical regularities in their environment, although they are not explicitly learning or applying statistics. Like the development of implicit theories, this concept of statistical learning counters the possible misconception of babies as passive learners and bears on the vital importance of their having opportunities to observe and interact with the environment. Several examples of statistical learning are provided in Box 4-2 .

Examples of Statistical Learning. Infants can use information about the statistics of syllables in the speech they hear to help them parse words. How do we know from hearing prettybaby that baby is more likely to be a word than tyba ? One way is that the (more...)

Understanding Causal Inference

Children's intuitive understanding of causal inference has long been recognized as a fundamental component of conceptual development. Young children, although not explicitly or consciously experimenting with causality, can experience observations and learning that allow them to conclude that a particular variable X causes (or prevents) an effect Y. Recent advances in the field have documented the ways young children can implicitly use the statistics of how events covary to infer causal relations, make predictions, generate explanations, guide their exploration, and enable them to intervene in the environment. The understanding of causal inference also provides an example of how different cognitive abilities—such as a sensitivity to statistical regularities and the development of implicit theories based on observation and learning (discussed in the two preceding sections and Box 4-2 )—interact with and can mutually support each other. There is now a substantial literature on young children's implicit ability to use what they observe in different conditions to understand the relations between variables. Several examples of young children developing the ability to understand causal inference are provided in Box 4-3 .

Examples of Understanding Causal Inference. One of the first studies of children's understanding of causal inference showed that children can rule out one variable and isolate another (Gopnik et al., 2001). Preschool children were presented with a machine (more...)

Sensitivity to Teaching Cues

Csibra and Gergely (2009) argue that humans are equipped with a capacity to realize when someone is communicating something for their benefit and that they construe that information differently than when they merely witness it. As noted previously in the discussion of developing theory of mind, children as early as infancy devote special attention to social situations that are likely to represent learning opportunities because adults communicate that intention. Information learned in such communicative contexts is treated as more generalizable and robust than that learned in a noncommunicative context.

In one study, for example, 9-month-old babies saw an adult either reach for an object (a noncommunicative act) or point to an object (a communicative act). The entire display was then screened from view, and after a brief delay, the curtains were opened, and babies saw either the same object in a new location or a new object in the same location. The short delay imposed a memory requirement, and for babies this young, encoding both the location and the identity of the object taxes their memory. The location of the object will typically be more salient and memorable to babies than the object's properties, but the prediction of this study was that babies who saw the adult point to the object would construe the pointing as a communicative act—“this adult is showing me something”—and would thus be more likely to encode the properties as opposed to the location of the object. Babies' looking times served as a measure of their surprise at or interest in an unexpected event. As predicted, babies appeared to encode different aspects of the event in the different conditions. When they had previously witnessed the adult reaching for the object, they were surprised when the object was in a new location but showed no renewed interest when there was a different object in the old location. In contrast, when babies first saw an adult point to the object, they were surprised when a new object appeared in the old location but not when the old object had changed locations ( Yoon et al., 2008 ).

Infants' Sensitivity to Teaching Cues: Implications for Adults

Babies have the capacity to realize when someone is communicating something for their benefit and therefore to construe information differently than when they merely witness it. When adults use face-to-face contact, call a baby's name, and point for the baby's benefit, these signals lead babies to recognize that someone is teaching them something, and this awareness can affect how and what they learn.

The significance of eye contact and other communication cues also is evident in research on whether, how, and when young children learn from video and other forms of digital media. Experiments conducted with 24-month-olds, for example, revealed that they can learn from a person on a video screen if that person is communicating with them through a webcam-like environment, but they showed no evidence of learning from a prerecorded video of that person. The webcam environment included social cues, such as back-and-forth conversation and other forms of social contact that are not possible in prerecorded video. Other studies found that toddlers learned verbs better during Skype video chats than during prerecorded video chats that did not allow for authentic eye contact or back-and-forth interaction ( Roseberry et al., 2014 ; Troseth et al., 2006 ). (See also Chapter 6 for more on technology and learning.)

The benefits of communicative pedagogical contexts for the conceptual development of preschool children also have been investigated. In one set of studies, 4-year-old children were exposed to a novel object's function either by seeing an adult deliberately use the object or by seeing the adult deliberately use the object after maintaining eye contact with the child and saying “watch this.” In both conditions, children noticed the object's property and attempted to elicit it from other similar objects. But when those objects were doctored to be nonfunctional, the children in the nonpedagogical condition quickly abandoned their attempts to elicit the property and played with the objects in some other way. Children who saw the same evidence but with direct communication for their benefit persisted in trying to elicit the property from other objects ( Butler and Markman, 2012a , b ). In other words, children's conviction that other similar objects should have the same unforeseen property was bolstered by their belief that the adult was performing the function for their benefit. Moreover the intentional (but nonpedagogical) condition versus the pedagogical condition produced strikingly different conceptions of the function ( Butler and Markman, 2014 ). Four- and 5-year-old children witnessed an object's function and were then given a set of objects to play with. Some objects were identical in appearance to the first object, while some differed in color (in one study) or shape (in another). Half of the objects of each color (or shape) had the unforeseen property, and half did not. Children were told they could play with the objects for a while and then should put them away in their appropriate boxes when done. The goal was to see whether children would sort the objects by the salient perceptual property (color or shape) or by function. Children in the pedagogical condition viewed the function as definitive and classified the objects by systematically testing each to see whether it had the function, while children in the nonpedagogical condition sorted by the salient color or shape. Thus, identical evidence is construed differently when children believe it has been produced for their benefit.

Effects of Adult Language on Cognition

Understanding the power of language is important for people who interact with children. Simple labels can help children unify disparate-looking things into coherent categories; thus labeling is a powerful way to foster conceptual development. Labels also can reify categories or concepts in ways that may or may not be intended. For example, frequently hearing “boys and girls” line up for recess, quiet down, etc. implicitly reinforces gender as an important dimension, compared with saying “children.” Box 4-4 presents examples of linguistic distinctions that affect children's construction of conceptual systems.

Examples of the Effects of Adult Language on Cognition. Some kinds of categories—two round balls, for example—are fairly easy to form, such that even babies treat the objects as similar. But many objects that adults view as members of (more...)

Effects of Language Used by Adults on Children's Cognitive Development: Implications for Adults

Awareness of the benefits and pitfalls of the language used by adults is important for people who interact with children. The language used by adults affects cognitive growth and learning in children in many subtle ways. Labeling is a powerful way to foster conceptual development. Simple labels can help children unify disparate things into coherent categories, but can also have the unintended consequence of reinforcing categories or concepts that are not desirable.

Conclusions About Cognitive Development and Early Learning Learning begins prenatally, and children are not only “ready to learn” but already actively learning from the time they are born. From birth, children's minds are active and inquisitive, and early thinking is insightful and complex. Many of the foundations of sophisticated forms of learning, including those important to academic success, are established in the earliest years of life. Development and early learning can be supported continuously as a child develops, and early knowledge and skills inform and influence future learning. When adults understand how the mind develops, what progress children make in their cognitive abilities, and how active inquiry and learning are children's natural inclination, they can foster cognitive growth by supporting children's active engagement with new experiences and providing developmentally appropriate stimulation of new learning through responsive, secure, and sustained caregiving relationships.

Implications for Care and Education Settings and Practitioners

The research findings on cognitive development in young children summarized above reflect an evolving understanding of how the mind develops during the early years and should be part of the core knowledge that influences how care and education professionals support young children's learning, as discussed in Chapter 7 . Many of these concepts describe cognitive processes that are implicit. By contrast with the explicit knowledge that older children and adults can put into words, implicit knowledge is tacit or nonconscious understanding that cannot readily be consciously described (see, e.g., Mandler, 2004 ). Examples of implicit knowledge in very young children include many of the early achievements discussed above, such as their implicit theories of living things and of the human mind and their nonconscious awareness of the statistical frequency of the associations among speech sounds in the language they are hearing. Infants' and young children's “statistical learning” does not mean that they can count, nor are their “implicit theories” consciously worked out. Not all early learning is implicit, of course. Very young children are taking significant strides in their explicit knowledge of language, the functioning of objects, and the characteristics of people and animals in the world around them. Thus early learning occurs on two levels: the growth of knowledge that is visible and apparent, and the growth of implicit understanding that is sometimes more difficult to observe.

This distinction between implicit and explicit learning can be confusing to early childhood practitioners (and parents), who often do not observe or recognize evidence for the sophisticated implicit learning—or even the explicit learning—taking place in the young children in their care. Many of the astonishingly competent, active, and insightful things that research on early cognitive development shows are going on in young children's minds are not transparent in their behavior. Instead, toddlers and young children seem highly distractable, emotional, and not very capable of managing their impulses. All of these observations about young children are true, but at the same time, their astonishing growth in language skills, their very different ways of interacting with objects and living things, and their efforts to share attention (such as through pointing) or goals (such as through helping) with an adult suggest that the cognitive achievements demonstrated in experimental settings have relevance to their everyday behavior.

This point is especially important because the cognitive abilities of young children are so easily underestimated. In the past, for example, the prevalent belief that infants lack conceptual knowledge meant that parents and practitioners missed opportunities to explore with them cause and effect, number, or symbolic play. Similarly, the view that young children are egocentric caused many adults to conclude that there was little benefit to talking about people's feelings until children were older—this despite the fact that most people could see how attentive young children were to others' emotions and how curious about their causes.

In light of these observations, how do early educators contribute to the cognitive growth of children in their first 3 years? One way is by providing appropriate support for the learning that is occurring in these very young children (see, e.g., Copple et al., 2013 ). Using an abundance of child-directed language during social interaction, playing counting games (e.g., while stacking blocks), putting into words what a classroom pet can do or why somebody looks sad, exploring together what happens when objects collide, engaging in imitative play and categorization (sorting) games—these and other shared activities can be cognitively provocative as long as they remain within the young child's capacities for interest and attention. They also build on understandings that young children are implicitly developing related to language; number; object characteristics; and implicit theories of animate and inanimate objects, physical causality, and people's minds. The purpose of these and other activities is not just to provide young children with cognitive stimulation, but also to embed that stimulation in social interaction that provokes young children's interest, elicits their curiosity, and provides an emotional context that enables them to focus their thinking on new discoveries. The central and consistent feature of all these activities is the young child's shared activity with an adult who thoughtfully capitalizes on his or her interests to provoke cognitive growth. The implications for instructional practices and curricula for educators working with infants and toddlers are discussed further in Chapter 6 .

Another way that educators contribute to the cognitive growth of infants and toddlers is through the emotional support they provide ( Jamison et al., 2014 ). Emotional support is afforded by the educator's responsiveness to young children's interests and needs (including each child's individual temperament), the educator's development of warm relationships with children, and the educator's accessibility to help when young children are exploring on their own or interacting with other children ( Thompson, 2006 ). Emotional support of this kind is important not only as a positive accompaniment to the task of learning but also as an essential prerequisite to the cognitive and attentional engagement necessary for young children to benefit from learning opportunities. Because early capacities to self-regulate emotion are so limited, a young child's frustration or distress can easily derail cognitive engagement in new discoveries, and children can lose focus because their attentional self-regulatory skills are comparably limited. An educator's emotional support can help keep young children focused and persistent, and can also increase the likelihood that early learning experiences will yield successful outcomes. Moreover, the secure attachments that young children develop with educators contribute to an expectation of adult support that enables young children to approach learning opportunities more positively and confidently. Emotional support and socioemotional development are discussed further later in this chapter.

The characteristics of early learning call for specific curricular approaches and thoughtful professional learning for educators, but it is also true that less formal opportunities to stimulate early cognitive growth emerge naturally in children's everyday interactions with a responsive adult. Consider, for example, a parent or other caregiver interacting with a 1-year-old over a shape-sorting toy. As they together are choosing shapes of different colors and the child is placing them in the appropriate (or inappropriate) cutout in the bin, the adult can accompany this task with language that describes what they are doing and why, and narrates the child's experiences of puzzlement, experimentation, and accomplishment. The adult may also be using number words to count the blocks as they are deposited. The baby's attention is focused by the constellation of adult behavior—infant-directed language, eye contact, and responsiveness—that signals the adult's teaching, and this “pedagogical orientation” helps focus the young child's attention and involvement. The back-and-forth interaction of child and adult activity provides stimulus for the baby's developing awareness of the adult's thinking (e.g., she looks at each block before commenting on it or acting intentionally on it) and use of language (e.g., colors are identified for each block, and generic language is used to describe blocks in general). In this interaction, moreover, the baby is developing both expectations for what this adult is like—safe, positive, responsive—and skills for social interaction (such as turn taking). Although these qualities and the learning derived from them are natural accompaniments to child-focused responsive social interaction with an adult caregiver, the caregiver's awareness of the child's cognitive growth at this time contributes significantly to the adult's ability to intentionally support new discovery and learning.

As children further develop cognitively as preschoolers, their growth calls for both similar and different behavior by the adults who work with them. While the educator's emotional support and responsiveness remain important, children from age 3 to 5 years become different kinds of thinkers than they were as infants and toddlers ( NRC, 2001 ). First, they are more consciously aware of their knowledge—much more of their understanding is now explicit. This means they are more capable of deliberately enlisting what they know into new learning situations, although they are not yet as competent or strategic in doing so as they will be in the primary grades. When faced with a problem or asked a question, they are more capable of offering an answer based on what they know, even when their knowledge is limited. Second, preschoolers are more competent in learning from their deliberate efforts to do so, such as trial-and-error or informal experimentation. While their success in this regard pales by comparison with the more strategic efforts of a grade-schooler, their “let's find out” approach to new challenges reflects their greater behavioral and mental competence in figuring things out. Third, preschoolers also are intuitive and experiential, learning by doing rather than figuring things out “in the head.” This makes shared activities with educators and peers potent opportunities for cognitive growth.

Nonetheless, the potential to underestimate the cognitive abilities of young children persists in the preschool and kindergarten years. In one study, for example, children's actual performance was six to eight times what was estimated by their own preschool teachers and other experts in consulting, teacher education, educational research, and educational development ( Claessens et al., 2014 ; Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 1996 ). Such underestimation represents a lost opportunity that can hinder children's progress. A study in kindergarten revealed that teachers spent most of their time in basic content that children already knew, yet the children benefited more from advanced reading and mathematics content ( Claessens et al., 2014 )—an issue discussed in depth in Chapter 6 . Unfortunately, when care and education professionals underestimate children's abilities to understand and learn subject-matter content, the negative impact is greatest on those with the fewest prior learning experiences ( Bennett et al., 1984 ; Clements and Sarama, 2014 ).

Conversely, when educators practice in a way that is cognizant of the cognitive progress of children at this age, they can more deliberately enlist the preschool child's existing knowledge and skills into new learning situations. One example is interactive storybook reading, in which children describe the pictures and label their elements while the adult and child ask and answer questions of each other about the narrative. Language and literacy skills also are fostered at this age by the adult's use of varied vocabulary in interaction with the child, as well as by extending conversation on a single topic (rather than frequently switching topics), asking open-ended questions of the child, and initiating conversation related to the child's experiences and interests ( Dickinson, 2003 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ; Dickinson and Tabors, 2001 ). In each case, dialogic conversation about text or experience draws on while also extending children's prior knowledge and language skills. Language and literacy skills are discussed further in a subsequent section of this chapter, as well as in Chapter 6 .

Another implication of these cognitive changes is that educators can engage preschool children's intentional activity in new learning opportunities. Children's interest in learning by doing is naturally suited to experimental inquiry related to science or other kinds of inquiry-based learning involving hypothesis and testing, especially in light of the implicit theories of living things and physical causality that children bring to such inquiry ( Samarapungavan et al., 2011 ). In a similar manner, board games can provide a basis for learning and extending number concepts. In several experimental demonstrations, when preschool children played number board games specifically designed to foster their mental representations of numerical quantities, they showed improvements in number line estimates, count-on skill, numerical identification, and other important quantitative concepts ( Laski and Siegler, 2014 ).

Other research has shown that instructional strategies that promote higher-level thinking, creativity, and even abstract understanding, such as talking about ideas or about future events, is associated with greater cognitive achievement by preschool-age children (e.g., Diamond et al., 2013 ; Mashburn et al., 2008 ). For example, when educators point out how cardinal numbers can be used to describe diverse sets of elements (four blocks, four children, 4 o'clock), it helps them generalize an abstract concept (“fourness”) that describes a set rather than the characteristics of each element alone. These activities also can be integrated into other instructional practices during a typical day.

Another implication of the changes in young children's thinking during the preschool years concerns the motivational features of early learning. Preschool-age children are developing a sense of themselves and their competencies, including their academic skills ( Marsh et al., 1998 , 2002 ). Their beliefs about their abilities in reading, counting, vocabulary, number games, and other academic competencies derive from several sources, including spontaneous social comparison with other children and feedback from teachers (and parents) concerning their achievement and the reasons they have done well or poorly. These beliefs influence, in turn, children's self-confidence, persistence, intrinsic motivation to succeed, and other characteristics that may be described as learning skills (and are discussed more extensively later in this chapter). Consequently, how teachers provide performance feedback to young children and support for their self-confidence in learning situations also is an important predictor of children's academic success ( Hamre, 2014 ).

In the early elementary years, children's cognitive processes develop further, which accordingly influences the strategies for educators in early elementary classrooms. Primary grade children are using more complex vocabulary and grammar. They are growing in their ability to make mental representations, but they still have difficulty grasping abstract concepts without the aid of real-life references and materials ( Tomlinson, 2014 ). This is a critical time for children to develop confidence in all areas of life. Children at this age show more independence from parents and family, while friendship, being liked and accepted by peers, becomes more important. Being in school most of the day means greater contact with a larger world, and children begin to develop a greater understanding of their place in that world ( CDC, 2014 ).

Children's growing ability to self-regulate their emotions also is evident in this period (discussed more extensively later in this chapter). Children understand their own feelings more and more, and learn better ways to describe experiences and express thoughts and feelings. They better understand the consequences of their actions, and their focus on concern for others grows. They are very observant, are willing to play cooperatively and work in teams, and can resolve some conflicts without seeking adult intervention ( CDC, 2014 ). Children also come to understand that they can affect others' perception of their emotions by changing their affective displays ( Aloise-Young, 1993 ). Children who are unable to self-regulate have emotional difficulties that may interfere with their learning. Just as with younger children, significant adults in a child's life can help the child learn to self-regulate ( Tomlinson, 2014 ).

Children's increasing self-regulation means they have a greater ability to follow instructions independently in a manner that would not be true of preschool or younger children. Educators can rely on the growing cognitive abilities in elementary school children in using instructional approaches that depend more independently on children's own discoveries, their use of alternative inquiry strategies, and their greater persistence in problem solving. Educators in these settings are scaffolding the skills that began to develop earlier, so that children are able to gradually apply those skills with less and less external support. This serves as a bridge to succeeding in upper primary grades, so if students lack necessary knowledge and skills in any domain of development and learning, their experience during the early elementary grades is crucial in helping them gain those competencies.

Building on many of the themes that have emerged from this discussion, the following sections continue by looking in more depth at cognitive development with respect to learning specific subjects and then at other major elements of development, including general learning competencies, socioemotional development, and physical development and health.

  • LEARNING SPECIFIC SUBJECTS

Interrelationships among different kinds of skills and abilities contribute to young children's acquisition of content knowledge and competencies, which form a foundation for later academic success. These skills and abilities include the general cognitive development discussed above, the general learning competencies that allow children to control their own attention and thinking; and the emotion regulation that allows children to control their own emotions and participate in classroom activities in a productive way (the latter two are discussed in sections later in this chapter). Still another important category of skills and abilities, the focus of this section, is subject-matter content knowledge and skills, such as competencies needed specifically for learning language and literacy or mathematics.

Content knowledge and skills are acquired through a developmental process. As children learn about a topic, they progress through increasingly sophisticated levels of thinking with accompanying cognitive components. These developmental learning paths can be used as the core of a learning trajectory through which students can be supported by educators who understand both the content and those levels of thinking. Each learning trajectory has three parts: a goal (to develop a certain competence in a topic), a developmental progression (children constructing each level of thinking in turn), and instructional activities (tasks and teaching practices designed to enable thinking at each higher level). Learning trajectories also promote the learning of skills and concepts together—an effective approach that leads to both mastery and more fluent, flexible use of skills, as well as to superior conceptual understanding ( Fuson and Kwon, 1992 ; National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008 ). See Chapter 6 for additional discussion of using learning trajectories and other instructional practices.

Every subject area requires specific content knowledge and skills that are acquired through developmental learning processes. It is not possible to cover the specifics here for every subject area a young child learns. To maintain a feasible scope, this chapter covers two core subject areas: (1) language and literacy and (2) mathematics. This scope is not meant to imply that learning in other areas, such as science, engineering, social studies, or the arts, is unimportant or less subject specific. Rather, these two were selected because they are foundational for other subject areas and for later academic achievement, and because how they are learned has been well studied in young children compared with many other subject areas.

Language and Literacy

Children's language development and literacy development are central to each other. The development of language and literacy includes knowledge and skills in such areas as vocabulary, syntax, grammar, phonological awareness, writing, reading, comprehension, and discourse skills. The following sections address the development of language and literacy skills, including the relationship between the two; the role of the language-learning environment; socioeconomic disparities in early language environments; and language and literacy development in dual language learners.

Development of Oral Language Skills

Language skills build in a developmental progression over time as children increase their vocabulary, average sentence length, complexity and sophistication of sentence structure and grammar, and ability to express new ideas through words ( Kipping et al., 2012 ). Catts and Kamhi (1999) define five features of language that both work independently and interact as children develop language skills: phonology (speech sounds of language), semantics (meanings of words and phrases), morphology (meaningful parts of words and word tenses), syntax (rules for combining and ordering words in phrases), and pragmatics (appropriate use of language in context). The first three parameters combined (phonology, semantics, and morphology) enable listening and speaking vocabulary to develop, and they also contribute to the ability to read individual words. All five features of language contribute to the ability to understand sentences, whether heard or read (O' Connor, 2014 ). Thus, while children's development of listening and speaking abilities are important in their own right, oral language development also contributes to reading skills.

Developing oral communication skills are closely linked to the interactions and social bonds between adults and children. As discussed earlier in this chapter, parents' and caregivers' talk with infants stimulates—and affects—language comprehension long before children utter their first words. This comprehension begins with pragmatics—the social aspects of language that include facial and body language as well as words, such that infants recognize positive (and negative) interactions. Semantics (understanding meanings of words and clusters of words that are related) soon follows, in which toddlers link objects and their attributes to words. Between the ages of 2 and 4, most children show dramatic growth in language, particularly in understanding the meanings of words, their interrelationships, and grammatical forms ( Scarborough, 2001 ).

Karmiloff and Karmiloff-Smith (2001) suggest that children build webs among words with similar semantics, which leads to broader generalizations among classes of related words. When adults are responsive to children's questions and new experiences, children expand their knowledge of words and the relationships among them. Then, as new words arise from conversation, storytelling, and book reading, these words are linked to existing webs to further expand the store of words children understand through receptive language and use in their own conversation. The more often adults use particular words in conversation with young children, the sooner children will use those words in their own speech ( Karmiloff and Karmiloff-Smith, 2001 ). Research has linked the size of vocabulary of 2-year-olds to their reading comprehension through fifth grade ( Lee, 2011 ).

One of the best-documented methods for improving children's vocabularies is interactive storybook reading between children and their caregivers (O' Connor, 2014 ). Conversations as stories are read improve children's vocabulary ( Hindman et al., 2008 ; Weizman and Snow, 2001 ), especially when children are encouraged to build on the possibilities of storybooks by following their interests ( Whitehurst et al., 1988 ; Zucker et al., 2013 ). Book reading stimulates conversation outside the immediate context—for example, children ask questions about the illustrations that may or may not be central to the story. This introduces new words, which children attach to the features of the illustrations they point out and incorporate into book-centered conversations. This type of language, removed from the here and now, is decontextualized language. Children exposed to experiences not occurring in their immediate environment are more likely to understand and use decontextualized language ( Hindman et al., 2008 ). Repeated routines also contribute to language development. As books are read repeatedly, children become familiar with the vocabulary of the story and their conversations can be elaborated. Routines help children with developmental delays acquire language and use it more intelligibly ( van Kleek, 2004 ).

Conversation around a story's content and emphasis on specific words in the text (i.e., the phonological and print features of words alongside their meanings) have long-term effects ( Zucker et al., 2013 ). The quality of adult readers' interactions with children appears to be especially important to children's vocabulary growth (see also Coyne et al., 2009 ; Justice et al., 2005 ). In a study with preschool children, Zucker and colleagues (2013) found that teachers' intentional talk during reading had a longer-lasting effect on the children's language skills than the frequency of the teachers' reading to the children. Moreover, the effect of the teachers' talk during reading was not moderated by the children's initial vocabulary or literacy abilities. The long-term effect of high-quality teacher–child book-centered interactions in preschool lasted through the end of first grade.

New research shows that the effects of interactive reading also hold when adapted to the use of digital media as a platform for decontextualized language and other forms of language development. A study of videobooks showed that when adults were trained to use dialogic questioning techniques with the videos, 3-year-olds learned new words and recalled the books' storylines ( Strouse et al., 2013 ). However, a few studies of e-books also have shown that the bells and whistles of the devices can get in the way of those back-and-forth conversations if the readers and the e-book designers are not intentional about using the e-books to develop content knowledge and language skills ( Parish-Morris et al., 2013 ). (See also the discussion of effective use of technology in instruction in Chapter 6 .)

Alongside developing depth of vocabulary (including the meaning of words and phrases and their appropriate use in context), other important parameters of language development are syntax (rules for combining and ordering words in phrases, as in rules of grammar) and morphology (meaningful parts of words and word tenses). Even before the age of 2, toddlers parse a speech stream into grammatical units ( Hawthorne and Gerken, 2014 ). Long before preschool, most children join words together into sentences and begin to use the rules of grammar (i.e., syntax) to change the forms of words (e.g., adding s for plurals or ed for past tense). Along with these morphemic changes to words, understanding syntax helps children order the words and phrases in their sentences to convey and to change meaning. Before children learn to read, the rules of syntax help them derive meaning from what they hear and convey meaning through speech. Cunningham and Zibulsky (2014 , p. 45) describe syntactic development as “the ability to understand the structure of a sentence, including its tense, subject, and object.”

Although syntactic understanding develops for most children through conversation with adults and older children, children also use these rules of syntax to extract meaning from printed words. This becomes an important reading skill after first grade, when text meaning is less likely to be supported with pictures. Construction of sentences with passive voice and other complex, decontextualized word forms are more likely to be found in books and stories than in directive conversations with young children. An experimental study illustrates the role of exposure to syntactic structures in the development of language comprehension ( Vasilyeva et al., 2006 ). Four-year-olds listened to stories in active or passive voice. After listening to ten stories, their understanding of passages containing these syntactic structures was assessed. Although students in both groups understood and could use active voice (similar to routine conversation), those who listened to stories with passive voice scored higher on comprehension of this structure.

Children's understanding of morphology—the meaningful parts of words—begins in preschool for most children, as they recognize and use inflected endings to represent verb tense (e.g., -ing, -ed, -s) and plurals, and continues in the primary grades as children understand and use prefixes and suffixes. By second and third grade, children's use of morphemes predicts their reading comprehension ( Nagy et al., 2006 ; Nunes et al., 2012 ).

Development of Literacy Skills

Literacy skills follow a developmental trajectory such that early skills and stages lead into more complex and integrated skills and stages ( Adams, 1990 ). For example, phonemic awareness is necessary for decoding printed words ( Ball and Blachman, 1991 ; Bradley and Bryant, 1983 ; O'Connor et al., 1995 ), but it is not sufficient. Students need to understand the alphabetic principle (that speech sounds can be represented by letters of the alphabet, which is how speech is captured in print) before they can use their phonemic awareness (the ability to hear and manipulate sounds in spoken words) to independently decode words they have never seen before ( Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1989 ; O'Connor and >Jenkins, 1995 ). Thus, instruction that combines skill development for 4- to 6-year-old children in phonemic awareness, letter knowledge, and conceptual understanding and use of these skills is more effective than teaching the skills in isolation ( Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1989 ; O'Connor and Jenkins, 1995 ).

Seminal theories and studies of reading describe an inextricable link between language development and reading achievement (e.g., Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1995 ; Gough and Tunmer, 1986 ; Hoover and Gough, 1990 ; Johnston and Kirby, 2006 ; Joshi and Aaron, 2000 ; Tunmer and Hoover, 1993 ; Vellutino et al., 2007 ). Early oral language competencies predict later literacy ( Pearson and Hiebert, 2010 ). Not only do young children with stronger oral language competencies acquire new language skills faster than students with poorly developed oral language competencies ( Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ), but they also learn key literacy skills faster, such as phonemic awareness and understanding of the alphabetic principle ( Cooper et al., 2002 ). Both of these literacy skills in turn facilitate learning to read in kindergarten and first grade. By preschool and kindergarten listening and speaking abilities have long-term impacts on children's reading and writing abilities in third through fifth grade ( Lee, 2011 ; Nation and Snowling, 1999 ; Sénéchal et al., 2006 ).

Vocabulary development (a complex and integrative feature of language that grows continuously) and reading words (a skill that most children master by third or fourth grade) ( Ehri, 2005 ) are reciprocally related, and both reading words accurately and understanding what words mean contribute to reading comprehension ( Gough et al., 1996 ). Because comprehending and learning from text depend largely upon a deep understanding of the language used to communicate the ideas and concepts expressed, oral language skills (i.e., vocabulary, syntax, listening comprehension) are at the core of this relationship between language and reading ( NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2005 ; Perfetti, 1985 ; Perfetti and Hart, 2002 ). For example, children with larger speaking vocabularies in preschool may have an easier time with phoneme awareness and the alphabetic principle because they can draw on more words to explore the similarities among the sounds they hear in spoken words and the letters that form the words ( Metsala and Walley, 1998 ). Each word a child knows can influence how well she or he understands a sentence that uses that word, which in turn can influence the acquisition of knowledge and the ability to learn new words. A stronger speaking and listening vocabulary provides a deeper and wider field of words students can attempt to match to printed words. Being bogged down by figuring out what a given word means slows the rate of information processing and limits what is learned from a sentence. Thus, differences in early vocabulary can have cascading, cumulative effects ( Fernald et al., 2013 ; Huttenlocher, 1998 ). The transition from speaking and listening to reading and writing is not a smooth one for many children. Although a well-developed vocabulary can make that transition easier, many children also have difficulty learning the production and meanings of words. Longitudinal studies of reading disability have found that 70 percent of poor readers had a history of language difficulties ( Catts et al., 1999 ).

Conclusion About the Development of Language and Literacy Skills The oral language and vocabulary children learn through interactions with parents, siblings, and caregivers and through high-quality interactions with educators provide the foundation for later literacy and for learning across all subject areas, as well as for their socioemotional well-being. The language interactions children experience at home and in school influence their developing minds and their understanding of concepts and ideas.

Role of the Language-Learning Environment

Today's science of reading development focuses more broadly than on teaching children to read the actual words on a page. As stressed throughout this report, young children's development entails a back-and-forth process of social interactions with knowledgeable others in their environment ( Bruner, 1978 ; NRC and IOM, 2000 ; Vygotsky, 1978 , 1986 ), and research has focused on the language of these interactions, examining how children's linguistic experiences influence aspects of their development over time, including their literacy development. The daily talk to which children are exposed and in which they participate is essential for developing their minds—a key ingredient for building their knowledge of the world and their understanding of concepts and ideas. In turn, this conceptual knowledge is a cornerstone of reading success.

The bulk of the research on early linguistic experiences has investigated language input in the home environment, demonstrating the features of caregivers' (usually the mother's) speech that promote language development among young children. The evidence accumulated emphasizes the importance of the quantity of communicative input (i.e., the number of words and sentences spoken) as well as the quality of that input, as measured by the variety of words and syntactic structures used (for relevant reviews, see Rowe, 2012 ; Vasilyeva and Waterfall, 2011 ). Because children's language development is sensitive to these inputs, variability in children's language-based interactions in the home environment explains some of the variance in their language development.

A smaller but growing and compelling research base is focused on how children's literacy skills are influenced by language use in early care and education settings and schools—for example, linguistic features of these settings or elementary school teachers' speech and its relationship to children's reading outcomes ( Greenwood et al., 2011 ). This research has particularly relevant implications for educational practices (discussed further in Chapter 6 ).

The language environment of the classroom can function as a support for developing the kind of language that is characteristic of the school curriculum—for example, giving children opportunities to develop the sophisticated vocabulary and complex syntax found in texts, beginning at a very early age ( Schleppegrell, 2003 ; Snow and Uccelli, 2009 ). Moreover, advances in cognitive science suggest that it is not enough to be immersed in environments that offer multiple opportunities for exposure to varied and rich language experiences. Rather, the process also needs to be socially mediated through more knowledgeable persons who can impart their knowledge to the learner; again, social interaction is a critical component of cognitive development and learning. Early childhood settings and elementary classrooms thus not only present opportunities for exposure to varied language- and literacy-rich activities (whether written or spoken), but also provide a person who is expert in mediating the learning process—the educator.

Research demonstrates that teachers' use of high-quality language is linked to individual differences in language and literacy skills; this work likewise shows the substantial variation in the quality of teacher talk in early childhood classrooms (e.g., Bowers and Vasilyeva, 2011 ; Gámez and Levine, 2013 ; Greenwood et al., 2011 ; Huttenlocher et al., 2002 ). For example, Huttenlocher and colleagues (2002) found greater syntactic skills in preschoolers exposed to teachers who used more syntactically complex utterances. Another study found for monolingual English-speaking children that fourth-grade reading comprehension levels were predicted by exposure to sophisticated vocabulary in preschool. These effects were mediated by children's vocabulary and literacy skills in kindergarten ( Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ).

In classroom studies focused on the linguistic environment, the level of analysis has involved broad measures of language use, such as amount of talk (i.e., teacher–student interactions by minute: Connor et al., 2006 ), amount of instruction (i.e., in teacher-managed versus child-managed instruction: Connor et al., 2007 ), type of interaction style (i.e., didactic versus cognitively demanding talk or the amount of extended discourse: Dickinson and Smith, 1991 ; Jacoby and Lesaux, 2014 ; Smith and Dickinson, 1994 ), or instructional moves made by the teacher (e.g., modeling: see review in Lawrence and Snow, 2011 ). A commonly included measurement that has been linked to children's literacy development is extended discourse, defined as talk that “requires participants to develop understandings beyond the here and now and that requires the use of several utterances or turns to build a linguistic structure, such as in explanations, narratives, or pretend” ( Snow et al., 2001 , p. 2). Children are better prepared to comprehend narrative texts they encounter in school if their early language environments provide more exposure to and opportunities to participate in extended discourse. This is because extended discourse and narrative texts share similar patterns for communicating ideas ( Uccelli et al., 2006 ).

Engaging groups of children in effective extended discourse involves asking and discussing open-ended questions and encouraging turn taking, as well as monitoring the group to involve nonparticipating children ( Girolametto and Weitzman, 2002 ). In addition to using interactive storybook and text reading as a platform for back-and-forth conversations (often referred to as interactive or dialogic reading, as described in the preceding section) ( Mol et al., 2009 ; Zucker et al., 2013 ), engaging children in extended discourse throughout classroom activities (e.g., small-group learning activities, transitions and routines [ van Kleek, 2004 ], dramatic play [ Mages, 2008 ; Morrow and Schickedanz, 2006 ]) is fundamental to providing a high-quality language-learning environment ( Jacoby and Lesaux, 2014 ).

In an example of the influence of the quantity and quality of teachers' language input in linguistically diverse classrooms, Bowers and Vasilyeva (2011) found that the total number of words produced by teachers and the diversity of their speech (which was entirely in English) were related to vocabulary gains for children from both English-only households and households in which English was not the primary language, respectively. Thus, they found that preschool dual language learners benefited only from increased quantities of language exposure and showed a negative relationship between vocabulary growth and teachers' syntactic complexity. By contrast, the English-only children—who presumably had more developed English language proficiency skills—benefited from the diversity of teachers' vocabulary and syntactic complexity. These findings are consistent with the notion that to promote language learning, different inputs are needed at different developmental stages ( Dickinson and Freiberg, 2009 ; Gámez and Lesaux, 2012 ). Children benefit from hearing simplified speech during very early word learning ( Furrow et al., 1979 ). With more exposure to language and more advanced vocabulary development, they benefit from speech input that is more complex (i.e., Hoff and Naigles, 2002 ). Hoff (2006) suggests that if input is too complex, children filter it out without negative consequences—as long as sufficient beneficial input is available to them. On the other hand, “children have no way to make up for input that is too simple” ( Hoff, 2006 , p. 75).

An important consideration in light of these findings is that recent research in early childhood classrooms serving children from low-income backgrounds suggests that daily high-quality language-building experiences may be rare for these children. For example, in a Head Start organization serving large numbers of Latino children a recent observational study found a preschool environment lacking in the frequent and high-quality teacher–child language interactions that are needed to support language and literacy development ( Jacoby and Lesaux, 2014 ). Literacy instruction was highly routine based and with low-level language structures. Extended discourse was infrequently used; only 22 percent of observed literacy-based lessons included at least one instance of extended discourse between a teacher and a child or group of children. Instead, teachers asked questions that yielded short answers or linked only to the here and now (e.g., What day is it today? What is the weather today? ). These features of infrequent extended discourse and predominantly routine-based literacy instruction were remarkably stable across teachers and classrooms. Other research investigating teacher talk in Head Start preschool classrooms has produced similar findings (e.g., Dickinson et al., 2008 ).

This is consistent with findings that there are sizable cultural and socioeconomic differences in high-quality language-promoting experiences in the home and in the classroom environment in early childhood ( Dickinson, 2003 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ; Dickinson and Tabors, 2001 ; Raikes et al., 2006 ), just as such differences have been found in the number of words children hear by the time they enter school ( Bradley and Corwyn, 2002 ; Fernald et al., 2013 ; Hart and Risley, 1995 ; Schneidman et al., 2013 ; Weisleder and Fernald, 2013 ). At the same time, for children from low-resource backgrounds oral language skills show an even stronger connection to later academic outcomes than for children from high-resource backgrounds. Given these findings, rich linguistic experiences at early ages may therefore be especially important for these children. Even small improvements in the literacy environment can have especially strong effects for children who are raised in low-income households ( Dearing et al., 2001 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ).

In sum, the language environment has important effects on children's learning, and children benefit from extensive opportunities to listen to and use complex spoken language ( National Early Literacy Panel, 2008 ). Teachers' use of high-quality language is linked to individual differences in language and literacy skills, and there is considerable variation in the quantity and quality of teachers' language use across classrooms. The quality of the classroom language environment is a lever for lasting improvements in children's language and literacy development, and it is important to tailor classroom talk to match the developmental stage of children's language acquisition.

Creating a Rich Language Environment: Implications for Adults

Improving language environments for young children requires daily learning opportunities that focus on the diversity and complexity of language used with young children. Practically speaking, this can be achieved through extended discourse, with multiple exchanges or turns that go beyond the immediate “here and now” using explanations, narratives, or pretend. Extended discourse can take place throughout all activities and in specific interactions, especially using book reading as a platform for back-and-forth conversations.

Further research is needed to advance understanding of language-based classroom processes and how dynamic and ongoing interactions facilitate or impede children's literacy. Such studies could advance existing research in at least two ways. In particular, it could further elucidate how language-based social processes in the classroom affect literacy development for the many students who enter schools and other care and education settings with limited proficiency in English. The majority of published studies focused on language-based interactions are focused on English-only learners, despite the fact that social processes can be experienced differently by different groups, even within the same setting ( Rogoff and Angelillo, 2002 ; Tseng and Seidman, 2007 ). Gámez and Levine (2013) suggest that future research examine the influence of dual-language input on dual language learners' language development; the nature of teacher talk during different parts of the instructional day, including joint book reading, and how these language experiences predict dual language learners' language skills; and the impact of classroom talk interventions—those that aim to manipulate the frequency and complexity of teachers' language—on both the language environment and dual language learners' language development.

In addition, prior research has measured a two-way process in a largely unidirectional manner—measuring speech only from parent to child or educator to student. It would be more valuable going forward if research were guided by the notion that the language-based interactions between students and educators mediate instruction, and were therefore to explore how communicative feedback loops, both adult–child and child–peer interactions, influence children's learning and development. Taking into account the student's contribution to the classroom language environment is particularly important in light of evidence that teachers modify their speech to conform to their students' limited language proficiency levels, potentially leading to a lower-quality language environment that impedes students' language growth ( Ellis, 2008 ; see Huttenlocher et al., 2010 ; Justice et al., 2013 ). More specifically, Justice and colleagues (2013) suggest that future research examine teacher–child language interactions in a multidimensional way to explore how syntactic complexity, cognitive demand, and even linguistic form (e.g., questions, comments) relate to each other; the links between children's use of complex syntax in classroom-based interactions and their future general language ability; and interventions designed to enhance classroom language interactions, focusing on both proximal and distal outcomes for children. Finally, greater understanding is needed of the ways in which the classroom language processes described in this section might act as a foundational mediator of the efficacy of interventions focused on learning outcomes in other domains and subject areas.

Alongside student–educator interactions, studies show that peer-to-peer interactions in the classroom may also have positive impacts on children's vocabulary and expressive language abilities. Children spend a significant amount of time interacting with other children in classroom settings, and a 2009 study examining the language growth and abilities of 4-year-olds in prekindergarten classrooms found that peers who have higher language abilities positively affect other children's language development. This study also found that children with advanced language skills will receive greater benefits from interacting with peers who also have advanced language skills ( Mashburn et al., 2009 ). These findings are similar to another study showing that peer interactions in the classroom, along with the ability level of the peers, have positive effects on the child's cognitive, prereading, expressive language skills ( Henry and Rickman, 2007 ). In order to achieve these benefits, however, the preschool classrooms need to be designed so that peers can interact with one another, and include activities such as reading books and engaging in play together. Children with teachers who organize the day with optimal amounts of time for peer-to-peer interactions may achieve greater language growth ( Mashburn et al., 2009 ).

Language and Literacy Development in Dual Language Learners 1

For children whose home language is not the predominant language of their school, educators and schools need to ensure the development of English proficiency. Both parents and preschool teachers can be particularly useful in improving these children's depth of vocabulary ( Aukrust, 2007 ; Roberts, 2008 ). At the same time, children can be helped to both build and maintain their first language while adding language and literacy skills in English ( Espinosa, 2005 ). In support of this as a long-term goal are the potential advantages of being bilingual, including maintaining a cultural and linguistic heritage and conferring an advantage in the ability to communicate with a broader population in future social, educational, and work environments. Additionally, an emerging field of research, albeit with mixed results to date, explores potential advantages of being bilingual that are linked more directly to cognitive development, starting in early childhood and extending to preserving cognitive function and delaying the symptoms of dementia in the elderly ( Bialystok, 2011 ; de Bruin et al., 2015 ).

Bilingual or multilingual children are faced with more communicative challenges than their monolingual peers. A child who frequently experiences failure to be understood or to understand may be driven to pay more attention to context, paralinguistic cues, and gestures in order to interpret an utterance, and thus become better at reading such cues. The result may be improved development of theory of mind and understanding of pragmatics ( Yow and Markman, 2011a , b ). In addition, the need to continually suppress one language for another affords ongoing practice in inhibitory or executive control, which could confer advantages on a range of inhibitory control tasks in children and helps preserve this fundamental ability in aging adults ( Bialystok, 2011 ; Bialystok and Craik, 2010 ; Bialystok et al., 2009 ).

One challenge in the education of dual language learners is that they sometimes are classified along with children with special needs. One reason for this is the lack of good assessment tools to help distinguish the nature of the difficulties experienced by dual language learners—whether due to a learning disability or to the fact that learning a second language is difficult, takes time, and develops differently in different children ( Hamayan et al., 2013 ).

Mathematics

Children's early knowledge of mathematics is surprisingly important, and it strongly predicts later success in mathematics ( Denton and West, 2002 ; Koponen et al., 2013 ; Passolunghi et al., 2007 ). Mathematics knowledge in preschool predicts mathematics achievement even into high school ( National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008 ; NRC, 2009 ; Stevenson and Newman, 1986 ). Mathematics ability and language ability also are interrelated as mutually reinforcing skills ( Duncan et al., 2007 ; Farran et al., 2005 ; Lerkkanen et al., 2005 ; O'Neill et al., 2004 ; Praet et al., 2013 ; Purpura et al., 2011 ). Indeed, mathematical thinking reaches beyond competence with numbers and shapes to form a foundation for general cognition and learning ( Clements and Sarama, 2009 ; Sarama et al., 2012 ), and problems with mathematics are the best predictor of failure to graduate high school. Mathematics therefore appears to be a core subject and a core component of thinking and learning ( Duncan and Magnuson, 2011 ; Duncan et al., 2007 ).

Given its general importance to academic success ( Sadler and Tai, 2007 ), children need a robust foundation in mathematics knowledge in their earliest years. Multiple analyses suggest that mathematics learning should begin early, especially for children at risk for later difficulties in school ( Byrnes and Wasik, 2009 ; Clements and Sarama, 2014 ). Well before first grade, children can learn the skills and concepts that support more complex mathematics understanding later. Particularly important areas of mathematics for young children to learn include number, which includes whole number, operations, and relations; geometry; spatial thinking; and measurement. Children also need to develop proficiency in processes for both general and specific mathematical reasoning ( NRC, 2009 ).

If given opportunities to learn, young children possess a remarkably broad, complex, and sophisticated—albeit informal—knowledge of mathematics ( Baroody, 2004 ; Clarke et al., 2006 ; Clements et al., 1999 ; Fuson, 2004 ; Geary, 1994 ; Thomson et al., 2005 ). In their free play, almost all preschoolers engage in substantial amounts of premathematical activity. They count objects; compare magnitudes; and explore patterns, shapes, and spatial relations. Importantly, this is true regardless of a child's income level or gender ( Seo and Ginsburg, 2004 ). Preschoolers can also, for example, learn to invent solutions to simple arithmetic problems ( Sarama and Clements, 2009 ).

High-quality mathematics education can help children realize their potential in mathematics achievement ( Doig et al., 2003 ; Thomson et al., 2005 ). However, without such education starting, and continuing throughout, the early years, many children will be on a trajectory in which they will have great difficulty catching up to their peers ( Rouse et al., 2005 ). As discussed further in Chapter 6 , early childhood classrooms typically are ill suited to helping children learn mathematics and underestimate their ability to do so. In some cases, children can even experience a regression on some mathematics skills during prekindergarten and kindergarten ( Farran et al., 2007 ; Wright, 1994 ). Mathematics needs to be conceptualized as more than skills, and its content as more than counting and simple shapes. Without building a robust understanding of mathematics in the early years, children too often come to believe that math is a guessing game and a system of rules without reason ( Munn, 2006 ).

Both education and experience can make a difference, as evidenced by data from the latest international Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study, which added data collection on early mathematics education ( Mullis et al., 2012 ). Students with higher mathematics achievement at fourth and sixth grades had parents who reported that they often engaged their children in early numeracy activities and that their children had attended preprimary education and started school able to do early numeracy tasks (e.g., simple addition and subtraction). Those children who had attended preschool or kindergarten had higher achievement, while the 13 percent who had attended no preprimary school had much lower average mathematics achievement ( Mullis et al., 2012 ).

Developmental Progression of Learning Mathematics

Children move through a developmental progression in specific mathematical domains, which informs learning trajectories as important tools for supporting learning and teaching. Recent work based on empirical research and emphasizing a cognitive science perspective conceptualizes learning trajectories for mathematics as “descriptions of children's thinking and learning in a specific mathematical domain, and a related, conjectured route through a set of instructional tasks designed to engender those mental processes or actions hypothesized to move children through a developmental progression of levels of thinking, created with the intent of supporting children's achievement of specific goals in that mathematical domain” ( Clements and Sarama, 2004 , p. 83).

Box 4-5 illustrates the concept of a developmental progression through the example of subitizing , an oft-neglected mathematical goal for young children. Research shows that subitizing, the rapid and accurate recognition of the number in a small group, is one of the main abilities very young children should develop ( Palmer and Baroody, 2011 ; Reigosa-Crespo et al., 2013 ). Through subitizing, children can discover critical properties of number, such as conservation and compensation ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ; Maclellan, 2012 ) and develop such capabilities as unitizing and arithmetic. Subitizing is not the only way children think and learn about number. Counting is the other method of quantification. It is the first and most basic mathematical algorithm and one of the more critical early mathematics competencies ( Aunola et al., 2004 ; National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008 ). Chapter 6 includes examples from a complete learning trajectory—goal, developmental progression, and instructional activities—for counting ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ).

Subitizing: A Developmental Progression. A quantitative, or numerical, “sense” is innate or develops early. For example, very young children possess approximate number systems (ANSs) that allow them to discriminate large and small sets, (more...)

Children with Special Needs

Children with special needs in learning mathematics fall into two categories. Those with mathematical difficulties struggle to learn mathematics for any reason; this category may apply to as many as 35-40 percent of students ( Berch and Mazzocco, 2007 ). Those with specific mathematics learning disabilities are more severe cases; these students have a memory or cognitive deficit that interferes with their ability to learn math ( Geary, 2004 ). This category may apply to about 6-7 percent ( Berch and Mazzocco, 2007 ; Mazzocco and Myers, 2003 ). In one study, this classification persisted in third grade for 63 percent of those classified as having mathematics learning disabilities in kindergarten ( Mazzocco and Myers, 2003 ).

Mathematics learning disabilities, while assumed to have a genetic basis, currently are defined by students' behaviors—yet with ongoing debate among experts about what those behaviors are. One consistent finding is that students with mathematics learning disabilities have difficulty retrieving basic arithmetic facts quickly. This has been hypothesized to be the result of an inability to store or retrieve facts and impairments in visual-spatial representation. As early as kindergarten, limited working memory and speed of cognitive processing may be problems for these children ( Geary et al., 2007 ). Many young children with learning disabilities in reading show a similar rapid-naming deficit for letters and words ( Siegel and Mazabel, 2013 ; Steacy et al., 2014 ). Another possibility is that a lack of higher-order, or executive, control of verbal material causes difficulty learning basic arithmetic facts or combinations. For example, students with mathematics learning disabilities may have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant associations. An illustration of this would be hearing “5 + 4” and saying “6” because it follows 5.

One explanation for the difficulty students with mathematics learning disabilities have learning basic arithmetic combinations might be delays in understanding counting. These students may not fully understand counting nor recognize errors in counting as late as second grade. They persist in using immature counting strategies, such as counting “one-by-one” on their fingers, throughout elementary school ( Geary et al., 1992 ; Ostad, 1998 ). Other experts, however, claim that a lack of specific competencies, such as subitizing, is more important ( Berch and Mazzocco, 2007 ).

Some evidence suggests that it is possible to predict which kindergartners are at risk for mathematics learning disabilities based on skill including reading numerals, number constancy, magnitude judgments of one-digit numbers, or mental addition of one-digit numbers ( Mazzocco and Thompson, 2005 ). However, until more is known, students should be classified as having mathematics learning disabilities only with great caution and after good mathematics instruction has been provided. Such labeling in the earliest years could do more harm than good ( Clements and Sarama, 2012 ).

Interrelationships Between Mathematics and Language

It can appear that language is less of a concern in mathematics compared to other subjects because it is assumed to be based on numbers or symbols, but this is not the case ( Clements et al., 2013a ). In fact, children learn math mainly from oral language, rather than from mathematical symbolism or textbooks ( Janzen, 2008 ). In addition, “talking math” is more than just using mathematics terms ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ). Therefore, both oral language and literacy in general, as well as the “language of mathematics,” are important for learning ( Vukovic and Lesaux, 2013 ). Vocabulary and knowledge of print are both predictors of later numeracy ( Purpura et al., 2011 ). Similarly, growth in mathematics from kindergarten to third grade is related to both early numerical skills and phonological processing ( Vukovic, 2012 ). In one study of linguistically and ethnically diverse children aged 6-9 years, language ability predicted gains in geometry, probability, and data analysis but not in arithmetic or algebra (controlling for reading ability, visual–spatial working memory, and gender) ( Vukovic and Lesaux, 2013 ). Thus, language may affect how children make meaning of mathematics but not its complex arithmetic procedures.

Moreover, there is an important bidirectional relationship between learning in mathematics and language ( Sarama et al., 2012 ). Each has related developmental milestones. Children learn number words at the same time as other linguistic labels. Most children recognize by the age of 2 which words are for numbers and use them only in appropriate contexts ( Fuson, 1988 ). Each also has related developmental patterns, with learning progressing along similar paths. In both, children recognize the whole before its parts. In learning language, this is word before syllable, syllable before rime-onset, and rime-onset before phoneme (see also Anthony et al., 2003 ; Ziegler and Goswami, 2005 ). Similarly in mathematics, numbers are first conceptualized as unbreakable categories and then later as composites (e.g., 5 is composed of 3 and 2) ( Butterworth, 2005 ; Sarama and Clements, 2009 ). By 6 years old in most cultures, children have been exposed to symbol representations that are both alphabetic and numerical, and they begin to be able to segment words into phonemes and numbers into singletons (e.g., understanding that 3 is 1 and 1 and 1) ( Butterworth, 2005 ; Sarama and Clements, 2009 ; Wagner et al., 1993 ). The ability to identify the component nature of words and numbers predicts the ability to read ( Adams, 1990 ; Stanovich and Siegel, 1994 ) and to compute ( Geary, 1990 , 1993 ). In addition to these similarities in typical developmental pathways, many children with learning disabilities experience deficits in competencies related to both language/literacy and numeracy ( Geary, 1993 ; Hecht et al., 2001 ; NRC, 1998 ).

Furthermore, there appear to be shared competencies between the two subject areas. For example, preschoolers' narrative abilities (i.e., their abilities to convey all the main events of a story and offer a perspective on its events) have been shown to predict mathematics achievement 2 years later ( O'Neill et al., 2004 ). Beginning mathematics scores have been shown to be highly predictive of subsequent achievement in both reading and mathematics although beginning reading skills (such as letter recognition, word identification, and word sounds) were shown to be highly predictive of later reading (advanced competencies such as evaluation) but not mathematics learning ( Duncan et al., 2007 ).

A causal relationship between rich mathematics learning and developing language and literacy skills is supported by a randomized study of the effects of a math curriculum called Building Blocks on prekindergarten children's letter recognition and oral language skills. Building Blocks children performed the same as the children in the control group on letter recognition and on three oral language subscales but outperformed them on four subscales: ability to recall key words, use of complex utterances, willingness to reproduce narratives independently, and inference ( Sarama et al., 2012 ). These skills had no explicit relation to the math curriculum. Similarly, a study of 5- to 7-year-olds showed that an early mathematics and logical-mathematical intervention increased later scores in English by 14 percentile points ( Shayer and Adhami, 2010 ).

Time on task (or time on instruction) does affect learning, which naturally leads to consideration of potential conflicts or tradeoffs between time spent on different subjects (e.g., Bodovski and Farkas, 2007 ). Indeed, a frequent concern is that introducing a mathematics curriculum may decrease the time devoted to language and literacy, impeding children's development in those areas, which are heavily emphasized in early learning goals (see Clements and Sarama, 2009 ; Farran et al., 2007 ; Lee and Ginsburg, 2007 ; Sarama and Clements, 2009 ). However, this assumes that mathematics activities will not have a positive effect on language and literacy. Yet as described here, evidence from both educational and psychological research suggests the potential for high-quality instruction in each to have mutual benefits for learning in both subjects. Rich mathematical activities, such as discussing multiple solutions and solving narrative story problems, can help lay the groundwork for literacy through language development, while rich literacy activities can help lay the groundwork for mathematics development ( Sarama et al., 2012 ).

Children Who Are Dual Language Learners

For mathematics learning in children who are dual language learners, the language, not just the vocabulary, of mathematics need to be addressed ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ). Challenges for dual language learners include both technical vocabulary, which can range in how similar or distinct terms are from everyday language, and the use of complex noun phrases. On the other hand, bilingual children often can understand a mathematical idea more readily because, after using different terms for it in different languages, they comprehend that the mathematical idea is abstract, and not tied to a specific term (see Secada, 1992 ).

There is evidence that the best approach is to teach these young children in their first language ( Celedón-Pattichis et al., 2010 ; Espada, 2012 ). At a minimum, their teachers need to connect everyday language with the language of math ( Janzen, 2008 ). It is also essential to build on the resources that bilingual children bring to learning mathematics—all cultures have “funds of knowledge” (culturally developed and historically accumulated bodies of knowledge and skills) that can be used to develop mathematical contexts and understandings ( Moll et al., 1992 ). Instructional practices for teaching mathematics with dual language learners are discussed further in Chapter 6 .

Conclusions About Learning Specific Subjects For subject-matter content knowledge and proficiency, children learn best when supported along a trajectory with three components: (1) their understanding of the subject-matter content itself, (2) their progress through predictable developmental levels and patterns of thinking related to their understanding of the content, and (3) instructional tasks and strategies that adults who work with children can employ to promote that learning at each level. For example: Almost all topics in mathematics follow predictable learning trajectories that include number counting and subitizing, number relationships and magnitude comparison, arithmetic operations, geometry and spatial sense, and measurement. Learning trajectories in literacy include specific developmental sequences in children's learning of phonological awareness and phonics (letter-sound correspondences), which together contribute to children's understanding of how spoken words are captured in reading and writing and thus to their advancement through broader levels of early literacy. Some principles of how children learn along a trajectory hold across subject-matter domains, but there are also substantive differences among subjects in the specific skills children need and in the learning trajectories. Both generalizable principles and subject-specific distinctions have implications for the knowledge and competencies needed to work with children. An important factor in children's learning of subject-matter content is how each of the components of learning trajectories both requires and develops aspects of learning that are not content specific, such as critical reasoning, executive function, self-regulation, learning skills, positive dispositions toward learning, and relationships.
  • GENERAL LEARNING COMPETENCIES

Educators, developmental scientists, and economists have long known that academic achievement is a result of both the growth of specific knowledge and the development of general learning competencies that regulate how children enlist cognitive resources when they encounter learning challenges, motivate advances in learning, and strengthen children's self-confidence as learners.

These general learning competencies have been labeled and categorized in various ways. Considerable recent research on some of these learning competencies has been conducted using the concept of “executive function,” which generally refers to a set of supervisory functions that regulate and control cognitive activity that affects learning ( Vitiello et al., 2011 ) and allow children to persevere with tasks, including learning tasks, even when facing fatigue, distraction, or decreased motivation. In the field of human development “mastery motivation” in infancy typically is indexed by the baby's persistence, focus, and curiosity in exploration and problem solving ( Morgan et al., 1990 ; Wang and Barrett, 2013 ). In preschool-age children, these skills often are conceptualized as the quality of the child's “approaches to learning,” which include motivation, engagement, and interest in learning activities. Heckman (2007) has used the term “noncognitive skills” to refer to many of these learning competencies, including self-control, persistence, self-discipline, motivation, and self-esteem, as well as future orientedness (i.e., the capacity to substitute long-term goals for immediate satisfactions). This label is used in contrast to the “cognitive skills” that are more often measured to predict children's later success, although there is considerable research that the “noncognitive skills” also support learning and achievement (see, e.g., Cunha and Heckman, 2010 ; Heckman, 2007 ), and they are highly relevant to cognitive skills in such areas as language, mathematics, science, and other traditional academic fields.

Here the alternative conceptualizations for these important aspects of child development and early learning are grouped as “learning competencies” to reflect their importance for early learning. Individual differences in these competencies are important determinants of learning and academic motivation, and children's experiences at home and in the classroom contribute to some of these differences. This section examines these competencies as well as their interrelationships with the previously discussed subject-matter domains of language and literacy and mathematics.

General Cognitive Skills

Several cognitive control processes are important for planning and executing goal-directed activity, which is needed for successful learning (e.g., Blair, 2002 ; Lyon and Krasnegor, 1996 ). These processes include, for example, short-term and working memory, attention control and shifting, cognitive flexibility (changing thinking between different concepts and thinking about multiple concepts simultaneously), inhibitory control (suppressing unproductive responses or strategies), and cognitive self-regulation. These processes also are closely related to emotion regulation, which is discussed later in the section on socioemotional development, and which also contributes to children's classroom success.

As noted previously, many general cognitive processes often are referred to collectively as “executive function,” although not everyone defines this construct in the same way (e.g., Miyake et al., 2000 ; Raver, 2013 ), and different disciplines and researchers differ as to which cognitive skills it includes. Other theoretical frameworks exist as well. For example, cognitive control and complexity theory postulates that executive function is an outcome, not an explanatory construct, and is the result of children's creation and application of rules (driven perhaps by an increase in reflection afforded by experience-dependent maturation of the prefrontal cortex) ( Müller et al., 2008 ; Zelazo and Carlson, 2012 ; Zelazo and Lyons, 2012 ). As with the overall domains of development displayed earlier in Figure 4-1 , the committee did not attempt to reconcile those different perspectives.

This variation in perspectives makes it difficult to parse the literature produced by different fields of research and practice. In general, however, executive function appears to improve most rapidly in young children ( Best et al., 2011 ; Blair, 2002 ; Hughes and Ensor, 2011 ; Romine and Reynolds, 2005 ; Schoemaker et al., 2014 ; Zelazo and Carlson, 2012 ). Executive function processes appear to be partially dependent on the development of the prefrontal cortex (the site of higher-order cognitive processes), notably through the preschool and kindergarten age range ( Bassett et al., 2012 ; Blair, 2002 ).

Short-Term and Working Memory

Short-term memory is the ability for short-term recall, such as of a sentence or important details from conversation and reading. Working memory allows children to hold in their memory information from multiple sources, whether heard or read, so they can use and link that information. Updating working memory is the ability to keep and use relevant information while engaging in another cognitively demanding task ( Conway et al., 2003 ; DeYoung, 2011 ).

Attention Control and Shifting

Attention control is the ability to focus attention and disregard distracting stimuli (e.g., a continuous performance task that requires a child to identify when some familiar object appears onscreen and ignore other objects that appear, or a task that requires ignoring extraneous information in a mathematics word problem). Attention shifting is a related process of switching a “mental set” while simultaneously ignoring distractions (e.g., counting by different units—tens and ones). Attention shifting and cognitive flexibility are often grouped.

Cognitive Flexibility

Cognitive flexibility capacities develop gradually throughout early childhood and have significant influences on children's social and academic competence. Cognitive flexibility is important, for example, for reading ( Duke and Block, 2012 ). Children who are better able to consider, at the same time, both letter-sound and semantic (meaning) information about words have better reading comprehension ( Cartwright, 2002 ; Cartwright et al., 2010 ). Reading comprehension also appears to improve when children are taught about words with multiple meanings (e.g., spell or plane ), and sentences with multiple meanings (e.g., “The woman chased the man on a motorcycle.”) ( Yuill, 1996 ; Zipke et al., 2009 ). In addition, interventions in young children that focus on cognitive flexibility have shown significant benefits for reading comprehension ( Cartwright, 2008 ).

Inhibitory Control

Inhibitory control involves controlling a dominant response (e.g., the first answer that comes to mind) so as to think about better strategies or ideas. The skill of simple response inhibition (withholding an initial, sometimes impulsive, response) develops during infancy through toddlerhood. Infants also develop some control of cognitive conflict in tasks in which an item of interest to them is first hidden in one location and then another, and the child must resist the response of searching in the first location ( Diamond, 1991 ; Müller et al., 2008 ; Rothbart and Rueda, 2005 ) (see Marcovitch and Zelazo, 2009 , for a model of possible mechanisms). Later in their first year, children can resolve conflict between their line of sight and their line of reaching ( Diamond, 1991 ). By about 30 months, they can successfully complete a spatial conflict task ( Rothbart and Rueda, 2005 ). From 3 to 5 years of age, complex response inhibition and response shifting develop, with attention shifting developing at about age 4 ( Bassett et al., 2012 ). The most rapid increase in inhibitory control is between 5 and 8 years of age, although moderate improvements are seen up to young adulthood ( Best et al., 2011 ).

Inhibitory control supports children's learning across subject-matter areas. As one example of its importance for mathematics, when the initial reading of a problem is not the correct one, children need to inhibit their impulse to answer (incorrectly) and carefully examine the problem. Consider the following problem: “There were six birds in a tree. Three birds already flew away. How many birds were there from the start?” Children have to inhibit the immediate desire to subtract prompted by the words “flew away” and perform addition instead.

Cognitive Self-Regulation

Cognitive self-regulation is what helps children plan ahead, focus attention, and remember past experiences. The construct of self-regulation and related concepts have a long history in psychology (e.g., Glaser, 1991 ; Markman, 1977 , 1981 ; Piaget and Szeminska, 1952 ; Sternberg, 1985 ; Vygotsky, 1978 ; Zelazo et al., 2003 ) and education (e.g., McGillicuddy-De Lisi, 1982 ; Steffe and Tzur, 1994 ). Most recently, researchers and educators have used the broad term self-regulation to refer to the processes involved in intentionally controlling attention, thinking, impulses, emotions, and behavior. In this way, self-regulation can be thought of in relation to several aspects of development, including the cognitive processes discussed here and the social and emotional processes discussed later in this chapter. Developmental psychobiological research and neuroimaging indicate that these subclasses are both neurally and behaviorally distinct while also being related and correlated ( Bassett et al., 2012 ; Hofmann et al., 2012 ; Hongwanishkul et al., 2005 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Willoughby et al., 2011 ). Together, these types of self-regulation allow children to persevere with tasks even when facing difficulties in problem solving or learning, fatigue, distraction, or decreased motivation ( Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ). It is thus unsurprising that kindergarten teachers believe self-regulation is as important as academics ( Bassok and Rorem, 2014 ).

Both cognitive self-regulation and emotional self-regulation (discussed later in this chapter) contribute to socioemotional development and also play a role in learning. Although the relationship between various features of cognitive self-regulation and academic achievement has been well documented for older students (e.g., Bielaczyc et al., 1995 ; Zimmerman, 2002 ), less was known until recently about how self-regulation developed in the early years contributes to the later development of cognitive and emotional self-regulation and academic achievement ( NRC and IOM, 2000 ).

Children's self-regulation and their ability to successfully function in school settings are related in two ways. First, emotional self-regulation enables children to benefit from learning in various social contexts, including their capacities to manage emotions in interactions with educators as well as peers (e.g., one-on-one, in cooperative pairs, in large and small groups). It also assists them in conforming to classroom rules and routines. Second, cognitive self-regulation enables children to develop and make use of cognitive processes that are necessary for academic learning ( Anghel, 2010 ).

Although most studies have focused on specific effects of either cognitive or emotional self-regulation, evidence suggests that the two are interconnected. This link is probably due to the commonality of the neurological mechanisms governing both emotional and cognitive self-regulation. For example, children lacking emotion regulation are likely also to have problems with regulating cognitive processes, such as attention ( Derryberry and Reed, 1996 ; LeDoux, 1996 ). Moreover, earlier patterns in the development of emotion control have been shown to be predictive of children's later ability to exercise control over their cognitive functioning ( Blair, 2002 ).

Several studies have shown positive correlations between self-regulation and achievement in young children (e.g., Bierman et al., 2008b ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Bull et al., 1999 ; Cameron et al., 2012 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Roebers et al., 2012 ; Welsh et al., 2010 ), although there are exceptions ( Edens and Potter, 2013 ). Preschoolers' cognitive self-regulation, including inhibitory control and attention shifting, were found to be related to measures of literacy and mathematics ability in kindergarten ( Blair and Razza, 2007 ). In another study, children with higher self-regulation, including attention, working memory, and inhibitory control, achieved at higher levels in literacy, language, and mathematics ( McClelland et al., 2007 ). Interventions in the area of self-regulation have shown positive effects for reading achievement ( Best et al., 2011 ; Bierman et al., 2008a ; Blair and Diamond, 2008 ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Diamond and Lee, 2011 ). Among struggling first graders in an effective reading intervention, those who were retained in grade showed significantly weaker self-regulation skills ( Dombek and Connor, 2012 ). Cognitive self-regulation appears to be strongly associated with academic learning ( Willoughby et al., 2011 ), but emotional self-regulation also contributes through children's adjustment to school and attitudes toward learning. In addition, both cognitive and emotional self-regulation contribute to variance in attention, competence motivation, and persistence ( Bassett et al., 2012 ; Willoughby et al., 2011 ).

In addition, differences in self-regulation competencies raise important issues related to disparities in educational achievement. Children in poverty can have lower self-regulation competencies (e.g., Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Bull and Scerif, 2001 ; Hackman and Farah, 2009 ; Jenks et al., 2012 ; Kishiyama et al., 2009 ; Masten et al., 2012 ; Mazzocco and Hanich, 2010 ; McLean and Hitch, 1999 ; Raver, 2013 ). One reason is the effect of chronic stress on behavioral and biological capacities for self-control (see discussion of chronic stress and adversity later in this chapter). This risk is exacerbated for children who are also dual language learners ( Wanless et al., 2011 ). Students with special needs are another population who may require focused interventions to develop self-regulation competencies ( Harris et al., 2005 ; Jenks et al., 2012 ; Lyon and Krasnegor, 1996 ; Mazzocco and Hanich, 2010 ; McLean and Hitch, 1999 ; Raches and Mazzocco, 2012 ; Toll et al., 2010 ; Zelazo et al., 2002 ). Students who are gifted and talented may also have exceptional needs in this domain (e.g., Mooji, 2010 ).

Adults who work with children have the opportunity to provide environments, experiences, and curricula that can help develop the competencies needed, including for children whose skills were not optimally developed in the earliest years. Importantly, the goal of such interventions is not to “train” children to suppress behaviors and follow rules. Rather, effective educators and programs provide learning activities and environments that increase children's capacity and disposition to set a goal (e.g., join a pretend play activity, complete a puzzle); develop a plan or strategy; and muster their social, emotional, and cognitive faculties to execute that plan. The science of how children develop and learn indicates that integrating academic learning and self-regulation is a sound approach.

Executive Functions and Learning in Specific Subjects

As already noted and shown in several examples, executive function processes are closely related to achievement in both language and literacy and mathematics ( Best et al., 2011 ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ), and this has also been shown in science ( Nayfeld et al., 2013 ). In some research, executive function has been correlated similarly with both reading and mathematics achievement across a wide age span (5 to 17 years), suggesting its significant role in academic learning ( Best et al., 2011 ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ). In contrast, some studies have found that executive function is more strongly associated with mathematics than with literacy or language ( Barata, 2010 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Ponitz et al., 2009 ; von Suchodoletz and Gunzenhauser, 2013 ). A strong relationship between executive function and mathematics may reflect that mathematics relies heavily on working memory and attention control, requiring the ability to inhibit an automatic response to a single aspect of a problem, to hold relevant information in mind, and to operate on it while shifting attention appropriately among different elements of a problem ( Welsh et al., 2010 ). This relationship is especially important given that mathematics curricula increasingly require higher-order skills, which executive function competencies provides ( Baker et al., 2010 ).

Some research indicates that most executive function competencies correlate significantly with mathematics achievement ( Bull and Scerif, 2001 ), while other studies suggest a greater role for particular executive function competencies in the learning of mathematics for young children—especially inhibitory control ( Blair and Razza, 2007 ) or working memory ( Bull et al., 2008 ; Geary, 2011 ; see also, Geary et al., 2012 ; cf. Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Szűcs et al., 2014 ; Van der Ven et al., 2012 ). These latter two competencies have been shown to predict success in mathematics in primary school students ( Toll et al., 2010 ). Working memory tasks have also been shown to predict mathematics learning disabilities, even more so than early mathematical abilities ( Toll et al., 2010 ). Several studies have identified lack of inhibition and working memory as specific deficits for children of lower mathematical ability, resulting in difficulty with switching to and evaluating new strategies for dealing with a particular task ( Bull and Scerif [2001] and Lan and colleagues [2011] found similar results). Persistence, another learning skill that is interrelated with cognitive processes, also has been linked to mathematics achievement for both 3- and 4-year-olds ( Maier and Greenfield, 2008 ).

Executive function competencies may be differentially associated with distinct areas of mathematics. For example, executive function was found to be correlated more with solving word problems than with calculation ( Best et al., 2011 ), and appears to play a role in acquiring new mathematics procedures and developing automatic access to arithmetic facts ( LeFevre et al., 2013 ). Different aspects of working memory also may be related to different mathematical areas ( Simmons et al., 2012 ). Parallel observations have been made for executive function and reading, with executive function playing a larger role in reading comprehension than in decoding.

In addition to the role of executive function in learning mathematics, mathematics activities also contribute to developing executive function. Some mathematics activities may require children to suppress prepotent responses, manipulate abstract information, and remain cognitively flexible. Importantly, neuroimaging studies suggest that executive function may be developed through learning mathematics in challenging activities but not in exercising mathematics once learned ( Ansari et al., 2005 ; Butterworth et al., 2011 ).

Cognitive Skills and Executive Function in Children with Special Needs

Some students with special needs may have a specific lack of certain executive function competencies ( Harris et al., 2005 ; Jenks et al., 2012 ; Lyon and Krasnegor, 1996 ; McLean and Hitch, 1999 ; Raches and Mazzocco, 2012 ; Schoemaker et al., 2014 ; Toll et al., 2010 ; Zelazo et al., 2002 ). Most of the research on executive function deficits in relation to disabilities that affect young children has focused on specific disorders, particularly attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). An early theory posited that ADHD is a lack of the behavioral inhibition required for proficiency with executive functions such as self-regulation of affect, motivation, and arousal; working memory; and synthesis analysis of internally represented information ( Barkley, 1997 ). Research has found that children diagnosed with ADHD are more likely than children without ADHD to have two or more deficits in executive function ( Biederman et al., 2004 ; cf. Shuai et al., 2011 ). A meta-analysis of studies of one measure of executive function, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, suggests that the performance of individuals with ADHD is fairly consistently poorer than that of individuals without clinical diagnoses ( Romine et al., 2004 ). In another study, children with ADHD were found not to have learning problems but rather problems in a measure of inhibitory control, which affected arithmetic calculation (as well as written language) ( Semrud-Clikeman, 2012 ). Other evidence suggests that children diagnosed with ADHD may have deficits not in executive processes themselves but in motivation or response to contingencies, that is, the regulation of effort allocation ( Huang-Pollock et al., 2012 ).

Having ADHD with deficits in executive function, compared to ADHD alone, is associated with an increased risk for grade retention and a decrease in academic achievement ( Biederman et al., 2004 ). The relationship between ADHD and executive functions may also depend on subtype. One study found that children with an inattention ADHD subtype showed deficits in several executive function competencies ( Tymms and Merrell, 2011 ), whereas children with the hyperactive-impulsive ADHD subtype may have fewer executive function deficits ( Shuai et al., 2011 ) and may even have strengths that could be developed in appropriate educational environments.

Deficits in executive function have been studied in other developmental disorders as well, albeit often in less detail. They include autism ( Bühler et al., 2011 ; Hill, 2004 ; Zelazo et al., 2002 ); attention and disruptive behavior problems ( Fahie and Symons, 2003 ; Hughes and Ensor, 2011 ); intellectual disabilities ( Nader-Grosbois and Lefèvre, 2011 ; Neece et al., 2011 ; Vieillevoye and Nader-Grosbois, 2008 ); cerebral palsy ( Jenks et al., 2012 ); Turner syndrome ( Mazzocco and Hanich, 2010 ); developmental dyslexia ( Brosnan et al., 2002 ; cf. Romine and Reynolds, 2005 ); and mathematics learning disabilities ( Toll et al., 2010 ).

Other Learning Skills and Dispositions

Other learning skills that are important to early academic achievement include persistence, curiosity, self-confidence, intrinsic motivation, time perspective (e.g., the willingness to prioritize long-term goals over immediate gratifications), and self-control. The growth of emotional and cognitive self-regulation is also fundamentally related to many of these developing learning skills. In addition, social experiences, discussed later in this chapter, are important for the growth of these learning skills. Note also that although these skills are referred to sometimes as dispositions, they are fostered through early experience and can be supported through intentional caregiving and instructional practices; they are not simply intrinsic traits in the child.

A capacity for focused engagement in learning is apparent from very early in life, although it is also true that these learning competencies develop significantly throughout early childhood as processes of neurobiological development interact with children's social experiences to enable greater persistence, focused attention, delayed gratification, and other components of effective learning and problem solving. As a consequence, very young children are likely to approach new learning situations with enthusiasm and self-confidence but at young ages may not necessarily bring persistence or creativity in confronting and solving challenging problems. Older preschoolers, by contrast, are more self-regulated learners. They approach new learning opportunities with initiative and involvement, and they are more persistent and more likely to solve problems creatively, by proposing their own ideas ( NRC, 2001 ).

Considerable research confirms the importance of these skills to early learning. Individual differences in infants' “mastery motivation” skills—persistence, focus, and curiosity in exploration and problem solving—predict later cognitive abilities and achievement motivation ( Busch-Rossnagel, 2005 ; Morgan et al., 1990 ; Wang and Barrett, 2013 ). In preschool-age children, learning skills that include motivation, engagement, and interest in learning activities have been found in longitudinal studies to predict children's cognitive skills at school entry ( Duncan et al., 2005 , 2007 ). Similarly, these characteristics continue to be associated with reading and mathematics achievement in the early elementary grades ( Alexander et al., 1993 ). Differences in these learning skills are especially associated with academic achievement for children in circumstances of economic disadvantage who face various kinds of self-regulatory challenges ( Blair and Raver, 2012 ; Howse et al., 2003a ).

Much of school success requires that children prioritize longer-term rewards requiring current effort over immediate satisfactions. The classic demonstration of this skill comes from a series of studies led by Walter Mischel beginning in the 1960s. Young children were offered the option of choosing an immediate, smaller reward or a larger reward if they waited to receive it later. For several years developmental outcomes for these children were tracked, which revealed that children who were better able to delay gratification at age 4 scored higher on measures of language skills, academic achievement, planful behaviors, self-reliance, capacity to cope with stress and frustration, and social competence measured in adolescence and adulthood ( Mischel et al., 1988 ). Other studies have reported consistent findings. Early development in the ability to prioritize future, long-term goals over short-term lesser gains improves children's chances of academic achievement and securing and maintaining employment ( Rachlin, 2000 ). Conversely, the inability to delay gratification is associated with young children's aggressive behavior, conduct problems, poorer peer relationships, and academic difficulty during preschool and the transition to elementary school ( Olson and Hoza, 1993 ) as well as later outcomes, including academic failure, delinquency, and substance abuse in adolescence ( Lynam et al., 1993 ; Wulfert et al., 2002 ).

The ways that children view themselves as learners are also important. Young children's self-perceived capability to master learning challenges develops early and exerts a continuing influence on their academic success. Early self-evaluations of competence are based on the positive and negative evaluations of children's behavior and competence by parents ( Stipek et al., 1992 ). Parent and educator expectations for children's success remain important. High parent expectations for children's school achievement are associated with children's later academic performance, and this is also true of educator expectations. In one longitudinal study, teacher expectations for children's math achievement in grades 1 and 3 directly predicted children's scores on standardized achievement tests 2 years later, and expectations for reading achievement had indirect associations with later reading scores. There was also evidence in this study that expectations were especially influential for academically at-risk students ( Hinnant et al., 2009 ).

Messages from parents and educators are also important in shaping how children attribute their own success and failure which, in turn, predicts their future effort and expectations of success. Children develop implicit theories in the early years about who they are as a person and what it means to be intelligent. Some children come to view intelligence as a fixed trait (i.e., one is either smart or not), whereas others see it as a more malleable trait that can be changed through effort and persistence. Educators and parents who approach learning goals by promoting and rewarding effort, persistence, and willingness to take on challenging problems increase children's motivation and their endorsement of effort as a path to success. In contrast, children receiving messages that intelligence is stable and cannot be improved through hard work are discouraged from pursuing difficult tasks, particularly if they view their abilities as low ( Heyman and Dweck, 1992 ). These patterns of “helpless” versus “mastery-oriented” motivation are learned in the preschool years and remain stable over time ( Smiley and Dweck, 1994 ).

These perceptions and patterns of motivation can be especially significant as children learn academic subjects, such as mathematics ( Clements and Sarama, 2012 ). People in the United States have many negative beliefs and attitudes about mathematics ( Ashcraft, 2006 ). One deeply embedded cultural belief is that achievement in mathematics depends mainly on native aptitude or ability rather than effort. Research shows that the belief in the primacy of native ability hurts students and, further, it is simply untrue.

Throughout their school careers, students who believe—or are helped to understand—that they can learn if they try working longer on tasks have better achievement than those who believe that either one “has it” (or “gets it”) or does not ( McLeod and Adams, 1989 ; Weiner, 1986 ). Researchers have estimated that students should be successful about 70 percent of the time to maximize motivation ( Middleton and Spanias, 1999 ). If students are directly assured that working hard to figure out problems, including making errors and being frustrated, are part of the learning process it can diminish feelings of embarrassment and other negative emotions at being incorrect. In contrast, students' learning can be impeded if educators define success only as rapid, correct responses and accuracy only as following the educator's example ( Middleton and Spanias, 1999 ). In addition, students will build positive feelings about mathematics if they experience it as a sense-making activity. Most young students are motivated to explore numbers and shapes and have positive feelings about mathematics ( Middleton and Spanias, 1999 ). However, after only a couple of years in typical schools, they begin to believe that only some people have the ability to do math.

A related pattern relating perceptions and emotions to learning is seen with students who experience mathematics anxiety. Primary grade students who have strong math anxiety, even alongside strong working memory, have been found to have lower mathematics achievement because working memory capacity is co-opted by math anxiety ( Beilock, 2001 ; Ramirez et al., 2013 ). Research has shown that primary grade students who “feel panicky” about math have increased activity in brain regions that are associated with fear, which decreases activity in brain regions associated with problem solving ( Young et al., 2012 ). Early identification and treatment of math anxiety may prevent children with high potential from avoiding mathematics and mathematics courses ( Ramirez et al., 2013 ).

  • SOCIOEMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

The development of social and emotional competence is an important part of child development and early learning. Socioemotional competence has been described as a multidimensional construct that contributes to the ability to understand and manage emotions and behavior; to make decisions and achieve goals; and to establish and maintain positive relationships, including feeling and showing empathy for others. Although their importance is widely recognized, universal agreement is lacking on how to categorize and define these areas of development. The Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning offers a summary construct with five interrelated groups of competencies that together encompass the areas typically considered to be part of socioemotional competence (see Figure 4-2 ).

Elements of socioemotional competence. SOURCE: Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (http://www.casel.org/social-and-emotional-learning/core-competencies, accessed March 24, 2015).

Socioemotional competence increasingly is viewed as important for a child's early school adjustment and for academic success at both the preschool and K-12 levels ( Bierman et al., 2008a , b ; Denham and Brown, 2010 ; Heckman et al., 2013 ; La Paro and Pianta, 2000 ; Leerkes et al., 2008 ). A growing body of research addresses the relationship between dimensions of socioemotional competence and cognitive and other skills related to early learning and later academic achievement ( Bierman et al., 2008a , b ; Graziano et al., 2007 ; Howse et al., 2003b ; Miller et al., 2006 ). Socioemotional development early in life also increasingly is understood to be critically important for later mental well-being, and for contributing to subsequent mental health problems when there are enduring disturbances in socioemotional functions ( IOM and NRC, 2009 ; Leckman and March, 2011 ).

There are several reasons why socioemotional development is important to early learning and academic success. As discussed in detail later in this section, early learning is a social activity in which these skills are important to the interactions through which learning occurs and is collaboratively shared. Socioemotional competence gives children the capacity to engage in academic tasks by increasing their ability to interact constructively with teachers, work collaboratively with and learn from peers, and dedicate sustained attention to learning ( Denham and Brown, 2010 ). Further, behavioral and emotional problems not only impede early learning but also pose other risks to long-term success. Substantial research has examined the relationship between delays and deficits in children's social skills and challenging behavior, such as serious problems getting along with peers or cooperating with educators ( Zins et al., 2007 ). When challenging behavior is not resolved during the early years, children with persistent early socioemotional difficulties experience problems in socialization, school adjustment, school success, and educational and vocational adaptation in adolescence and adulthood (e.g., Dunlap et al., 2006 ; Lane et al., 2008 ; Nelson et al., 2004 ). Thus attention to socioemotional competence also is important from the perspective of addressing early emerging behavior problems before they become more serious.

A variety of evidence-based approaches can be implemented to strengthen socioemotional competence for young children ( Domitrovich et al., 2012 ; IOM and NRC, 2009 ). These approaches typically entail strategies designed to improve children's emotion identification and understanding combined with the development of social problem-solving skills; practice in simple emotion regulation strategies; and coaching in prosocial behavior through strategies that can involve role playing, modeling, and reinforcement of socially competent behavior. Importantly, as discussed further in Chapter 6 , these strategies can be incorporated into daily classroom practice to provide children with everyday socioemotional learning.

Relational Security and Emotional Well-Being

As noted earlier in the discussion of self-regulation, socioemotional competencies contribute to the development of relationships with parents, educators, and peers. The development of positive relationships enables young children to participate constructively in learning experiences that are inherently social. The emotional support and security provided by positive relationships contributes in multifaceted ways to young children's learning success. Research on the security of attachment between young children and their parents illustrates this point, and provides a basis for considering the nature of children's relationships with educators and peers.

A secure parent–child attachment is widely recognized as foundational for healthy development, and the evolving understanding of the importance of attachment encompasses research in developmental psychology and developmental neuroscience (as discussed in Chapter 3 ) ( Schore and Schore, 2008 ; Thompson, 2013 ). Research has shown that securely attached children receive more sensitively responsive parental care, and in turn develop greater social skills with adults and peers and greater social and emotional understanding of others, show more advanced moral development, and have a more positive self-concept (see Thompson, 2013 , for a review). Securely attached children also have been found to be more advanced in cognitive and language development and to show greater achievement in school ( de Ruiter and van IJzendoorn, 1993 ; van Ijzendoorn et al., 1995 ; West et al., 2013 ). This association has been found for infants, preschool-age children, and older children, suggesting that it is fairly robust.

Most researchers believe that the association between attachment security and cognitive competence derives not from a direct link between the two, but from a number of processes mediating a secure attachment and the development of cognitive and language skills ( O'Connor and McCartney, 2007 ). The mediators that have been studied include the following:

  • Early confidence and competence at exploration—One of the functions of a secure attachment is to enable infants and young children to better explore the environment, confident in the caregiver's support and responsiveness if things go awry. An extensive research literature, focused primarily on young children, confirms this expectation ( van Ijzendoorn et al., 1995 ). Early in life, exploratory interest is likely to lead to new discoveries and learning.
  • Maternal instruction and guidance—Consistent with the sensitivity that initially contributes to a secure attachment, considerable research has shown that the mothers of securely attached children continue to respond supportively in ways that promote the child's social and cognitive achievements ( Thompson, in press) . In particular, these mothers talk more elaboratively with their children in ways that foster the children's deeper understanding and in so doing help support the children's cognitive growth ( Fivush et al., 2006 ). Furthermore, increased mother–child conversation is likely to foster the child's linguistic skills.
  • Children's social competence with adults and peers—Securely attached children develop enhanced social skills and social understanding that enhance their competence in interactions with peers and adults in learning environments. In this light, their greater cognitive and language competencies may derive, at least in part, from more successful interactions with social partners in learning contexts. (See the detailed discussion of social interaction as a forum for cognitive growth later in this section.)
  • Self-regulatory competence—Several studies suggest that securely attached children are more skilled in the preschool and early grade school years at self-regulation, especially as it is manifested in greater social competence and emotion regulation. Self-regulatory competence also may extend to children's greater attentional focus, cognitive self-control, and persistence in learning situations. In one recent report, the association of attachment security with measures of school engagement in the early primary grades was mediated by differences in children's social self-control; attentional impulsivity also varied with the security of attachment ( Drake et al., 2014 ; Thompson, 2013 ).
  • Stress management—One of the functions of a secure attachment is that it supports the social buffering of stress by providing children with an adult who regularly assists them in challenging circumstances. The social buffering of stress may be an especially important aspect of how a secure attachment contributes to cognitive competence for children in disadvantaged circumstances when stress is likely to be chronic and potentially overwhelming (see Gunnar and Donzella, 2002 , for a review; Nachmias et al., 1996 ) (see also the discussion of chronic stress and adversity later in this chapter).

In addition to the substantial research on parent–child attachment and the development of cognitive competence, a smaller but significant research literature focuses on the development of attachments between children and educators and how those attachments contribute to children's success in structured learning environments (e.g., Ahnert et al., 2006 ; Birch and Ladd, 1998 ; Howes and Hamilton, 1992 ; Howes et al., 1998 ; Ladd et al., 1999 ; Mitchell-Copeland et al., 1997 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004a , b ). In some respects, the processes connecting children's learning achievement with the supportive, secure relationships they develop with educators are similar to those observed with parent–child attachments. As with their parents and other caregivers, children develop attachments to their educators, and the quality of those relationships has a significant and potentially enduring influence on their classroom success ( Hamre and Pianta, 2001 ). Secure, warm relationships with educators facilitate young children's self-confidence when learning and assist in their self-regulatory competence, and there is evidence that children with such relationships in the classroom learn more than those who have more difficult relationships with educators ( NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2003 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ).

In one study, preschoolers identified as academically at risk based on demographic characteristics and reports of problems by their kindergarten teachers were followed to the end of first grade ( Hamre and Pianta, 2005 ). The children with first-grade teachers who provided high amounts of instructional and emotional support had achievement scores comparable to those of their low-risk peers. Support was measured by teacher behaviors such as verbal comments promoting effort, persistence, and mastery; conversations using open-ended questions; encouragement of child responsibility; sensitivity; and a positive classroom climate. O'Connor and McCartney (2007) likewise found that positive educator–child relationships from preschool through third grade were associated with higher third-grade achievement, and that much of this achievement derived from how positive relationships promoted children's classroom engagement.

Positive educator–child relationships are especially important during the transition to school, when children's initial expectations about school and adjustment to its social demands take shape ( Ladd et al., 1999 ; Silver et al., 2005 ). Children who develop more positive relationships with their teachers in kindergarten are more positive about attending school, more excited about learning, and more self-confident. In the classroom they achieve more compared with children who experience more conflicted or troubled relationships with their teachers ( Birch and Ladd, 1997 ; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2003 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ). A positive relationship with educators may be especially important for children who are at risk of academic difficulty because such a relationship can provide support for self-confidence and classroom involvement ( Pianta et al., 1995 ).

A similar association is seen for peer relationships. Children who experience greater friendship and peer acceptance tend to feel more positive about coming to school, participate more in activities in the classroom, and achieve more in kindergarten ( Ladd et al., 1996 , 1997 ). Peer rejection is associated with less classroom participation, poorer academic performance, and a desire to avoid school ( Buhs and Ladd, 2001 ).

Taken together, research documenting the association between the security of attachment and the development of cognitive and language competence, as well as the stronger academic performance of securely attached children, highlights the multiple ways in which supportive relationships contribute to early learning. In particular, such relationships with parents, educators, and even peers provide immediate support that helps children focus their energies on learning opportunities, and they also foster the development of social and cognitive skills that children enlist in learning.

Emotion Regulation and Self-Management

Another element of socioemotional competence, touched on earlier in the section on general learning competencies, is self-regulation of emotion, or emotion regulation, which can affect learning behaviors and relationships with adults and peers. As noted in that earlier discussion, emotion regulation is closely intertwined with cognitive self-regulation and executive function. Emotion regulation processes include emotional and motivational responses to situations involving risk and reward (e.g., Kerr and Zelazo, 2004 ). They are frequently inhibitory; that is, they include the ability to suppress one response (e.g., grabbing a toy from another) so as to respond in a better way (asking for or sharing the toy). The development of emotion regulation and other forms of self-management in the early years is based on slowly maturing regions of the prefrontal cortex that continue to develop throughout adolescence and even early adulthood. Thus, early learners are maturationally challenged to manage their attention, emotions, and behavioral impulses effectively in a care setting or classroom.

Because they have difficulty cooperating or resolving conflicts successfully, children who lack effective self-regulation do not participate in a productive way in classroom activities—including learning activities ( Broidy et al., 2003 ; Ladd et al., 1999 ; Saarni et al., 1998 ). Children with poor emotion regulation skills may act disruptively and aggressively; they then receive less support from their peers, which in turn may undermine their learning ( Valiente et al., 2011 ). Poor emotion regulation also diminishes positive educator–child interactions, which, as discussed in the previous section, has been shown to predict poor academic performance and behavior problems ( Hamre and Pianta, 2001 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Raver and Knitzer, 2002 ).

Coupled with joint attention and delay of gratification, self-regulation skills are linked to social competence and ease the transition to kindergarten ( Huffman et al., 2000 ; McIntyre et al., 2006 ). Children with difficulty regulating emotion in preschool and kindergarten often display inappropriate behavior, fail to pay attention (affecting whether they recall and process information), and have difficulty following instructions, all of which contribute to learning problems ( Eisenberg et al., 2010 ). Unfortunately, these difficulties tend to be common in preschool and kindergarten. They are an important determinant of whether educators and parents regard young children as “ready for school” ( Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2000 ).

Some researchers also suggest that emotion regulation in preschool and kindergarten serves as an early indicator of later academic success ( Graziano et al., 2007 ; Howse et al., 2003b ; Trentacosta and Izard, 2007 ). In preschool, McClelland and colleagues (2007) found not only that emotion regulation predicted early skills in literacy and mathematics but also that growth in emotion regulation in 4-year-olds over a 1-year period was linked to greater gains in literacy, vocabulary, and math compared with children showing less growth. Reading disability and problem behavior may be a “chicken or egg” problem: students who have behavior problems in first grade are more likely to have reading difficulties in third grade and students who have reading difficulties in first grade are more likely to exhibit behavior problems in third grade ( Morgan et al., 2008 ). Thus a particularly effective learning environment may be one that provides both effective reading instruction and support for behavioral self-regulation ( Connor et al., 2014 ).

Young children are better enabled to exercise self-regulation in the company of educators who have developmentally appropriate expectations for their self-control, provide predictable routines, and offer guidance that scaffolds their developing skills of self-management, especially in the context of carefully designed daily practices in a well-organized setting ( Bodrova and Leong, 2012 ). Indeed, in an intervention for academically at-risk young children, the Chicago School Readiness Project gave Head Start teachers specialized training at the beginning of the year in classroom management strategies to help lower-income preschoolers better regulate their own behavior. At the end of the school year, these children showed less impulsiveness, fewer disruptive behaviors, and better academic performance compared with children in classrooms with teachers who received a different training regimen ( Raver et al., 2009 , 2011 ).

Conclusion About the Ability to Self-Regulate The ability to self-regulate both emotion and cognitive processes is important for learning and academic achievement, affecting children's thinking, motivation, self-control, and social interactions. Children's progress in this ability from birth through age 8 is influenced by the extent to which relationships with adults, learning environments, and learning experiences support this set of skills, and their progress can be impaired by stressful and adverse circumstances.

Social and Emotional Understanding

As described earlier in this chapter, even infants and toddlers have an implicit theory of mind for understanding how certain mental states are associated with people's behavior. From their simple and straightforward awareness that people act intentionally and are goal directed; that people have positive and negative feelings in response to things around them; and that people have different perceptions, goals, and feelings, young children develop increasingly sophisticated understanding of the mental experiences that cause people to act as they do ( Wellman, 2011 ). They realize, for example, that people's beliefs about reality can be accurate or may be mistaken, and this realization leads to their understanding that people can be deceived, that the child's own thoughts and feelings need not be disclosed, and that not everybody can be believed ( Lee, 2013 ; Mills, 2013 ). They appreciate that people's thinking may be biased by expectations, prior experiences, and desires that cause them to interpret the same situation in very different ways ( Lalonde and Chandler, 2002 ). They also begin to appreciate how personality differences among people can cause different individuals to act in the same situation in very different ways ( Heyman and Gelman, 1999 ).

These remarkable advances in social understanding are important to children's developing socioemotional skills for interacting with educators and peers. These advances also are fostered by children's classroom experiences. Children learn about how people think and feel from directly observing; asking questions; and conversing about people's mental states with trusted informants, such as parents ( Bartsch and Wellman, 1995 ; Dunn, 2002 ; Thompson et al., 2003 ). Similarly, interactions with educators and peers provide young children with apt lessons in mutual understanding and perspective taking, cooperation, conflict management, personality differences and similarities, and emotional understanding in an environment where these skills are developing. This is especially so when educators can use children's experiences as forums for developing social and emotional understanding, such as when they explain why peers are feeling the way they do, suggest strategies for resolving conflict over resources or a point of view, or involve children in collective decision making involving different opinions.

Self-Awareness and Early Learning

How young children think of themselves as learners, and in particular their self-perceived efficacy in mastering new understanding, is an early developing and continuously important influence on their academic success. Young children become increasingly sensitive to the positive and negative evaluations of their behavior by parents, which serve as the basis for their self-evaluations ( Stipek et al., 1992 ). In one study, mothers who provided positive evaluations, gentle guidance, and corrective feedback during teaching tasks with their 2-year-olds had children who, 1 year later, were more persistent and less likely to avoid difficult challenges. By contrast, mothers who were intrusively controlling of their toddlers had children who, 1 year later, responded with shame when they had difficulty ( Kelley et al., 2000 ). Gunderson and colleagues (2013) found that 14- to 38-month-old children whose parents praised their efforts during unstructured home observations were more likely, as third graders, to believe that abilities are malleable and can be improved.

An extensive research literature documents the effects of parents' and educators' performance feedback on children's self-concept and motivation to succeed. Most of this research was conducted with older children and adolescents because of their more sophisticated understanding of differences in ability (see Wigfield et al., 2006 , for a review); however, preschoolers and early primary grade students are also sensitive to success and failure and to their imputed causes. In a study by Cimpian and colleagues (2007) , for example, 4-year-old children were represented by puppets whose performance was praised by a teacher using either generic feedback (“You are a good drawer.”) to imply trait-based (ability-centered) success or nongeneric feedback (“You did a good job drawing.”) to imply situation-based (effort-centered) success. The children did not differ in their self-evaluations after hearing praise of either kind, but when their puppet subsequently made a mistake and was criticized for it, the 4-year-olds who had heard generic feedback evaluated their performance and the situation more negatively than did children hearing nongeneric feedback, suggesting that they interpreted criticism as reflecting deficits in their ability. Similar results have been reported with kindergarteners by Kamins and Dweck (1999) and by Zentall and Morris (2010) , with the latter indicating that task persistence as well as self-evaluation were strengthened by the use of nongeneric performance feedback.

Parent and educator expectations for children's academic success also are important influences. High parental expectations for children's school achievement are associated with children's later academic performance, and this association often is mediated by the greater involvement of parents in the preschool or school program and other practices that support children's school success ( Baroody and Dobbs-Oates, 2009 ; Englund et al., 2004 ; Mantzicopoulos, 1997 ). The role of educator expectations in children's success is illustrated by a longitudinal study in which teacher expectations for children's math achievement in grades 1 and 3 directly predicted children's scores on standardized achievement tests 2 years later; teacher expectations for reading achievement had indirect associations with later reading scores. The results of this study also suggest that teacher expectations were especially influential for academically at-risk students ( Hinnant et al., 2009 ).

Social Interaction as a Forum for Cognitive Growth

A wider perspective on the importance of socioemotional skills for academic success is gained by considering the importance of social experiences for early learning. Contemporary research has led developmental scientists to understand the mind's development as deriving jointly from the child's naturally inquisitive activity and the catalysts of social experience. Sometimes these social experiences are in formal teaching and other pedagogical experiences, but often they take the form of adults and children sharing in activities that provide the basis for early learning, in a kind of “guided participation” (e.g., Rogoff, 1991 ). These activities can be as simple as the one-sided “conversation” parents have with their infant or toddler from which language skills develop, or the shared sorting of laundry into piles of similar color, or labeling of another child's feelings during an episode of peer conflict. In short, considerable early learning occurs in the course of a young child's ordinary interactions with a responsive adult.

Social experiences provide emotional security and support that enables learning and can also contribute to the development of language, number skills, problem solving, and other cognitive and learning skills that are foundational for school readiness and academic achievement. Through their interactions with children, adults provide essential stimulation that provides rapidly developing mental processes with catalysts that provoke further learning. Conversely, the lack of these catalysts contributes to learning disparities by the time that children become preschoolers. These processes are well illustrated by considering the growth of language and literacy skills and of mathematical understanding.

It is difficult to think of any child developing language apart from social interactions with others. As discussed earlier in this chapter, variability in these experiences, beginning in infancy, helps account for socioeconomic disparities in language and mathematical skills that are apparent by the time children enter school. In a widely cited study, Hart and Risley (1995) recorded 1 hour of naturally occurring speech in the homes of 42 families at monthly intervals beginning when children were 7-9 months old and continuing until they turned 3 years. They found that by age 3, children living in the most socioeconomically advantaged families had a working vocabulary that was more than twice the size of that of children growing up in the most disadvantaged families. The latter group of children also was adding words more slowly than their advantaged counterparts. The differences in children's vocabulary size were associated, in part, with how many words were spoken to them during the home observations, with a much richer linguistic environment being characteristic of the most advantaged homes. In addition, words were used in functionally different ways, with a much higher ratio of affirmative-to-prohibitive language being used in the most advantaged homes and a much lower ratio (i.e., below 1) being characteristic of the most disadvantaged homes. Differences in the language environment in which children grew up were, in other words, qualitative as well as quantitative in nature. Further research with a subset of 29 families in this sample showed that 3-year-olds' vocabulary size significantly predicted their scores on standardized tests of language skill in third grade ( Hart and Risley, 1995 ).

A later study by Fernald and colleagues (2013) confirms and extends these findings. A sample of 48 English-learning infants from families varying in socioeconomic status was followed from 18 to 24 months. At 18 months, significant differences between infants from higher- and lower-income families were already seen in vocabulary size and in real-time language processing efficiency. By 24 months, a 6-month gap was found between the two groups in processing skills related to language development. A companion study by Weisleder and Fernald (2013) with 29 lower-income Spanish-speaking families found that infants who experienced more child-directed speech at 19 months had larger vocabularies and greater language processing efficiency at 24 months. But adult speech that was simply overheard by infants (i.e., not child directed) at 19 months had no association with later language ( Schneidman et al., 2013 ). These studies indicate that child-directed speech, and perhaps the social interaction that accompanies it, is what strengthens infants' language processing efficiency. As in the Hart and Risley (1995) study, differences in family language environments were both qualitative and quantitative in nature. These findings are important in light of the association between the socioeconomic status of children's families and their language skills ( Bradley and Corwyn, 2002 ).

The findings of these studies are consistent with those of studies of the social experiences in and outside the home that promote language learning in early childhood. (See also the section on language and literacy under “Learning Specific Subjects” earlier in this chapter.) According to one longitudinal study, language and literacy skills in kindergarten were predicted by several aspects of the language environment at home and in classrooms in the preschool years. The characteristics of adult language that stimulated young children's language development included adult use of varied vocabulary during conversations with children; extended discourse on a single topic (rather than frequent topic switching); and diversity of language-related activities, including storybook reading, conversation related to children's experiences and interests, language corrections, and pretend play ( Dickinson, 2003 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ; Dickinson and Tabors, 2001 ). These elements of the early childhood social environment predicted both kindergarten language skills and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Other studies show that extensive use of descriptive language (e.g., labeling and commenting on people's actions) related to the child's current experience contributes to the quality of children's language development. Shared storybook reading also has been found to enhance the language skills of young children in lower-income homes ( Raikes et al., 2006 ). Stated differently, what matters is not just how much language young children are exposed to but the social and emotional contexts of language shared with an adult.

Language and literacy development is a major focus of instruction in prekindergarten and K-3 classrooms, and the instructional strategies used by teachers are both more formal and more sophisticated than those used in early childhood classrooms. Duke and Block (2012) have noted that in primary grade classrooms, vocabulary, reading comprehension, and conceptual and content knowledge are not adequately emphasized. The practices that would enhance early reading skills are embedded in children's social experiences with educators and peers in the classroom. They involve children interacting with partners throughout reading activity, and teachers explaining and discussing vocabulary terms and encouraging children to make personal connections with the concepts in the text.

Number Concepts and Mathematics

Language and literacy skills are the best-studied area in which early social experiences are influential, but they are not the only skills for which social interactions are important. Social experiences also are important for mathematics, such as for developing an understanding of numbers as well as early number and spatial/geometric language. Infants have an approximate number system that enables them to distinguish different quantities provided that the numerical ratio between them is not small, and this discrimination ability improves with increasing age (see Box 4-5 earlier in this chapter). There is some evidence that early individual differences in this ability are consistent during the first year and predict later mathematical abilities, although the reason for this remains unclear ( Libertus and Brannon, 2010 ; Starr et al., 2013 ). Toddlers also are beginning to comprehend certain number principles, such as one-to-one correspondence ( Slaughter et al., 2011 ). How adults talk about number is important. In one study, everyday parent–child discourse was recorded for 90 minutes every 4 months when the child was between 14 and 30 months old. The amount of parents' spontaneous “number talk” in these conversations (e.g., counting objects, references to time) was predictive of children's cardinal number knowledge (i.e., the knowledge that “four” refers to sets with four items) at 46 months ( Levine et al., 2010 ). Particularly important was when parents counted or labeled fairly large sets of objects within the child's view, providing concrete referents for parent–child interaction over number ( Gunderson and Levine, 2011 ).

Klibanoff and colleagues (2006) found that in early childhood, teachers' “math language”—that is, the frequency of their verbal references to number and geometric concepts—varied greatly for different teachers, but it significantly predicted progress in preschoolers' mathematical knowledge over the course of the school year. Similarly, another study found that parents' number-related activities at home with their young children were highly variable, but parents who engaged in more of these activities had children with stronger mathematical skill on standardized tests ( Blevins-Knabe and Musun-Miller, 1996 ). These practices in the classroom and at home help explain the significant socioeconomic disparities in number understanding by the time children arrive at school ( Klibanoff et al., 2006 ; Saxe et al., 1987 ). In addition to spoken references to numerical and geometric concepts, adults stimulate developing mathematical understanding when they incorporate these concepts into everyday activities, including games and other kinds of play; prompt children's explanations for numerical inferences; probe their understanding; and relate mathematical ideas to everyday experience ( NRC, 2009 ). Unfortunately, the quality of mathematical instruction is highly variable in preschool and early primary grades (discussed further in Chapter 6 ).

Taken together, these studies suggest the diverse ways in which social experiences provide catalysts for children's developing language and number skills that are the focus of later academic work. In these domains, adult practices provide essential cognitive stimulants beginning in infancy. Similar practices—adapted to young children's developing skills—remain important as children proceed through the primary grades.

Relationships and Early Learning: Implications for Adults

The relationship of an adult to a child—the emotional quality of their interaction, the experiences they have shared, the adult's beliefs about the child's capabilities and characteristics—helps motivate young children's learning, inspire their self-confidence, and provide emotional support to engage them in new learning.

Commonplace interactions provide contexts for supporting the development of cognitive and learning skills and the emotional security in which early learning thrives. Applauding a toddler's physical skills or a second-grader's writing skills, counting together the leaves on the sidewalk or the ingredients of a recipe, interactively reading a book, talking about a sibling's temper tantrum or an episode of classroom peer conflict—these and other shared experiences contribute to young children's cognitive development and early learning.

Conclusion About Socioemotional Development Socioemotional development contributes to the growth of emotional security that enables young children to fully invest themselves in new learning and to the growth of cognitive skills and competencies that are important for learning. These capacities are essential because learning is inherently a social process. Young children's relationships—with parents, teachers, and peers—thus are central to the learning experiences that contribute to their later success.
  • PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT AND HEALTH

Child development and early learning are closely intertwined with child health. Indeed, each is a foundation for outcomes in the other: health is a foundation for learning, while education is a determinant of health ( Zimmerman and Woolf, 2014 ). The Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University (2010) has described three foundational areas of child health and development that contribute to physical and mental well-being:

  • Stable and responsive relationships—Such relationships provide young children with consistent, nurturing, and protective interactions with adults that enhance their learning and help them develop adaptive capacities that promote well-regulated stress response systems.
  • Safe and supportive physical, chemical, and built environments—Such environments provide physical and emotional spaces that are free from toxins and fear, allow active exploration without significant risk of harm, and offer supports for families raising young children.
  • Sound and appropriate nutrition—Such nutrition includes health-promoting food intake as well as eating habits, beginning with the future mother's nutritional status even before conception.

This section examines interrelated topics of physical development, child health, nutrition, and physical activity and then touches on partnerships between the health and education sectors (also discussed in Chapter 5 ).

Physical Development

Physical development goes hand-in-hand with cognitive development in young children, and progress in one domain often relies on progress in the other. Similar to cognitive development, typical physical development follows a common trajectory among children but with individual differences in the rate of development. A child's physical development encompasses healthy physical growth; the development of sensory systems, including vision and hearing; and development of the ability to use the musculoskeletal system for gross motor skills that involve large body movements as well as fine motor skills that require precision and the controlled production of sound for speaking. Sensory and motor development are critical for both everyday and classroom activities that contribute to cognitive development, early learning, and eventually academic achievement.

Young children's growth in gross and fine motor skills develops throughout the birth through age 8 continuum—early on from holding their head up; rolling over; standing, crawling, and walking; to grasping cereal, picking up blocks, using a fork, tying shoelaces, and writing. A number of recent studies have focused on the relationships among the development of fine and gross motor skills in infants and young children, cognitive development, and school readiness. For example, one study found that students showing deficiencies in fine motor skills exhibited lower math and verbal scores ( Sandler et al., 1992 ), and more recent studies have also shown that fine motor skills were strongly linked to later achievement ( Grissmer et al., 2010a ; Pagani and Messier, 2012 ). Some of the same neural infrastructure in the brain that controls the learning process during motor development are also involved in the control of learning in cognitive development ( Grissmer et al., 2010a ). The evidence of the impact of motor skills on cognitive development and readiness for school calls for a shift in curricula to include activities that focus on fine motor skills, to include the arts, physical education, and play ( Grissmer et al., 2010b ).

Child Health

Health has an important influence on early learning and academic achievement. Hair and colleagues (2006) found that poor health can be as important in contributing to struggles with academic performance in first grade as language and cognitive skills, along with lack of social skills. Not only are healthy children better prepared to learn, but participation in high-quality early childhood programs leads to improved health in adulthood, setting the stage for intergenerational well-being. Data from Head Start and from the Carolina Abecedarian Project indicate that high-quality, intensive interventions can prevent, or at least delay, the onset of physical and emotional problems from adolescence into adulthood ( Campbell et al., 2014 ; Carneiro and Ginja, 2012 ). Data from a national longitudinal survey show that involvement in Head Start was associated with fewer behavior problems and serious health problems, such as 29 percent less obesity in males at 12 and 13 years of age. In addition, Head Start participants had less depression and obesity as adolescents and 31 percent less involvement in criminal activity as young adults. Similarly, long-term follow-up of adults who were enrolled in the Carolina Abecedarian Project revealed that males in their mid-30s in the project had lower rates of hypertension, obesity, and metabolic syndrome than controls. None of the males in the project had metabolic syndrome, compared with 25 percent of the control group. Further analysis of growth parameters indicated that those who were obese in their mid-30s were on that trajectory by 5 years of age, indicating the need for emphasis on healthy nutrition and regular physical activity beginning in early childhood. These studies suggest that the impact of early care and education programs on physical and emotional health is long term.

Sufficient, high-quality dietary intake is necessary for children's health, development, and learning. Support for providing healthy nutrition for children and their families, including pregnant and expectant mothers, is vital. Adequate protein, calories, and nutrients are needed for brain development and function. While the rapid brain growth and development that occurs in infants and toddlers may make children in this age group particularly vulnerable to dietary deficiencies, nutrition remains important as certain brain regions continue to develop through childhood into adolescence.

Nutrients, Cognitive Development, and Academic Performance

Deficiencies in protein, energy, and micronutrients such as iron, zinc, selenium, iodine, and omega-3 fatty acids have been linked to adverse effects on cognitive and emotional functioning ( Bryan et al., 2004 ). Research has shown that iron-deficiency anemia (IDA) is associated with lower cognitive and academic performance ( Bryan et al., 2004 ; Nyaradi et al., 2013 ; Taras, 2005 ). Children at an early school age who had IDA as an infant were found to have lower test scores than those who did not have IDA. Effects of severe IDA in infancy have been seen in adolescence. These effects include lower scores in motor functioning; written expression; arithmetic achievement; and some specific cognitive processes, such as spatial memory and selective recall ( NRC and IOM, 2000 ). However, it is not clear whether children with iron deficiency but no anemia have similar outcomes ( Taras, 2005 ). A review of daily iron supplementation in children aged 5-12 years studied in randomized and quasi-randomized controlled trials showed improvement in measures of attention and concentration, global cognitive scores, and, for children with anemia, intelligence quotient (IQ) scores ( Low et al., 2013 ).

IDA in infancy also has been associated with impaired inhibitory control and executive functioning. Altered socioemotional behavior and affect have been seen in infants with iron deficiency regardless of whether anemia is present ( Lozoff, 2011 ). One study found an association between iron supplementation in infancy and increased adaptive behavior at age 10 years, especially in the areas of affect and response to reward, which may have beneficial effects on school performance, mental health, and personal relationships ( Lozoff et al., 2014 ).

Folate and iodine also have been shown to be important for brain development and cognitive performance ( Bougma et al., 2013 ; Bryan et al., 2004 ; Nyaradi et al., 2013 ), although iodine deficiency is rare in the United States. While there is some evidence that zinc, vitamin B 12 , and omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids also may be important for cognitive development, the research on these associations is inconclusive ( Bougma et al., 2013 ; Bryan et al., 2004 ; Taras, 2005 ).

Food Insecurity, Diet Quality, and Healthful Eating

Food insecurity and diet quality in children have both been linked to impaired academic performance and cognitive and socioemotional development. Food insecurity refers to circumstances in which households do not have adequate food to eat, encompassing both inadequate quantity and nutritional quality of food ( ERS, 2014 ). Food insecurity affects development not only by compromising nutrition but also by contributing to a factor in family stress ( Cook and Frank, 2008 ). In 2012, 48 million Americans were food insecure, a fivefold increase from the 1960s and a 57 percent increase from the late 1990s. One in six Americans reported being short of food at least once per year. More than half of affected households were white, and more than half lived outside cities. Indeed, hunger in the suburbs has more than doubled since 2007. Two-thirds of food-insecure households with children have at least one working adult, typically in a full-time job ( McMillan, 2014 ).

A recent review indicates that food insecurity is a “prevalent risk to the growth, health, cognitive, and behavioral potential of low-income children” ( Cook and Frank, 2008 , p. 202). Studies found that children in food-insufficient families were more likely than those in households with adequate food to have fair/poor health; iron deficiency; and behavioral, emotional, and academic problems. Infants and toddlers are at particular risk from food insecurity even at its least severe levels ( Cook and Frank, 2008 ). Cross-sectional studies of children from developing countries have shown an association among general undernutrition and stunting, IQ scores, and academic performance ( Bryan et al., 2004 ). Alaimo and colleagues (2001) found that food insecurity was linked to poorer academic and psychosocial outcomes in children ages 6 to 11 years. Similarly, Florence and colleagues (2008) observed that students with lower overall diet quality were significantly more likely to fail a literacy assessment. Subsequent research has shown that while food insecurity experienced earlier in childhood was associated with emotional problems that appeared in adolescence, cognitive and behavioral problems could be accounted for by differences in the home environments, such as family income and the household's sensitivity to children's needs ( Belsky et al., 2010 ).

Eating breakfast, which can be related to food insecurity, diet quality, and healthful eating habits, has been associated with improved cognitive function, academic performance, and school attendance ( Basch, 2011 ; Hoyland et al., 2009 ; Mahoney et al., 2005 ; Nyaradi et al., 2013 ; Rampersaud et al., 2005 ). According to two reviews of the effect of consuming breakfast in children and adolescents, the evidence suggests that children who consume breakfast—particularly those children whose nutritional status is compromised—may have improved cognitive function, test grades, and school attendance. The positive effects of school breakfast programs may be explained in part by their effect of increasing school attendance ( Hoyland et al., 2009 ; Rampersaud et al., 2005 ). The composition of the breakfast meal may also be important to cognitive performance; a breakfast meal with a low glycemic index, such as oatmeal, has been shown to improve cognitive function ( Cooper et al., 2012 ; Mahoney et al., 2005 ).

In 2011, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) published a report documenting the relationship between healthy eating and increased life expectancy; improved quality of life; and fewer chronic diseases, including cardiovascular disease, obesity, metabolic syndrome, diabetes, and inadequate bone health ( CDC, 2011 ). The report documents the high rate of iron deficiency among obese children and emphasizes the link between dental caries and unhealthy diet. Children are unlikely to follow recommendations for the number of servings of various food groups and they consume higher-than-recommended amounts of saturated fats, sodium, and foods with added sugar. Children's eating behavior and food choices are influenced not only by taste preferences but also by the home environment and parental influences, including household eating rules, family meal patterns, and parents' lifestyles. The school environment influences children's eating behavior as well. The availability of unhealthy options in schools leads to poor choices by children, whereas research has shown that efforts to reduce the availability of sugar-sweetened beverages in the schools can have a positive impact on children's choices ( AAP Committee on School Health, 2004 ). There are also rising concerns about food insecurity in association with obesity; inexpensive foods tend not to be nutritious, and contribute to increasing rates of obesity ( IOM, 2011 ; McMillan, 2014 ).

Physical Activity

A recent Institute of Medicine (IOM) study linked increasing physical activity and enhancing physical fitness to improved academic performance, and found that this can be facilitated by physical activity built into children's days through physical education, recess, and physical classroom activity ( IOM, 2013 ). Likewise, the American Academy of Pediatrics recently highlighted the crucial role of recess as a complement to physical education, suggesting that recess offers cognitive, social, emotional, and physical benefits and is a necessary component of a child's development ( AAP Council on School Health, 2013 ). However, fewer than half of youth meet the current recommendation of at least 60 minutes of vigorous- or moderate-intensity physical activity per day, and recent years have seen a significant downward trend in the offering of daily physical education in schools at all levels ( CDC, 2012 ; GAO, 2012 ). Positive support from friends and family encourages children to engage in physical activity, as do physical environments that are conducive to activity. However, the school environment plays an especially important role. The IOM report recommends that schools provide access to a minimum of 60 minutes of vigorous- or moderate-intensity physical activity per day, including an average of 30 minutes per day in physical education class for students in elementary schools ( IOM, 2013 ).

Partnerships Between Health and Education

Each of the domains of child development and early learning discussed in this chapter can be supported through interventions that involve both the health and education sectors (see also the discussion of continuity among sectors in Chapter 5 ). Specific activities include coordinating vision, hearing, developmental, and behavioral screening to facilitate early identification of children with special needs; completing daily health checks; making appropriate referrals and collaborating with the child's medical home and dental health services; ensuring that immunizations for the entire family and for the early care and education workforce are up to date; modifying and adapting services to meet the individual needs of the child; and providing support to the early care and education workforce to promote more inclusive practices for children with special needs. In addition, teaching and modeling skills in sanitation and personal hygiene will contribute to preventing illness. Furthermore, pediatric health care professionals can make an important contribution by promoting literacy. Extensive research documents the positive impact on early language and literacy development when a pediatric professional gives advice to parents about reading developmentally appropriate books with children as early as 6 months of age ( AAP Council on Early Childhood et al., 2014 ).

There is evidence that coordinated efforts between educational settings and health care services lead to improved health. Head Start, the Infant Health and Development Program, and the Carolina Abecedarian Project are examples of early care and education programs that have integrated health care services into the intervention design, leading to positive health outcomes. Schools also can partner with pediatric health care professionals in their communities to identify opportunities to enhance physical activity in the school setting ( AAP Committee on Sports Medicine and Fitness and AAP Committee on School Health, 2000 ). CDC (2011) has offered recommendations for promoting healthful eating and physical activity that include the following and, if placed in an appropriate developmental context, can be applied to care and education settings for children aged 0-8:

  • Use a coordinated approach to develop, implement, and evaluate healthful eating and physical activity policies and practices.
  • Establish school environments that support healthy eating and activity.
  • Provide a quality school meal program and ensure that students have only appealing, healthy food and beverage choices offered outside of the school meal program.
  • Implement a comprehensive physical activity program with quality physical education as the cornerstone.
  • Implement health education that provides students with the knowledge, attitudes, skills, and experiences needed for healthy eating and physical activity.
  • Provide students with health, mental health, and social services to address healthy eating, physical activity, and related chronic disease prevention.
  • Partner with families and community members in the development and implementation of healthy eating and physical activity policies, practices, and programs.
  • Provide a school employee wellness program that includes healthy eating and physical activity services for all school staff members.
  • Employ qualified persons and provide them with professional development opportunities in staffing physical education; health education; nutrition services; health, mental health, and social services; and supervision of recess, cafeteria time, and out-of-school-time programs.

School-based health centers are another approach to partnering between health and education. They have been associated with improved immunization rates, better adherence to scheduled preventive examinations, and more treatment for illnesses and injuries, as well as fewer emergency room visits. For example, King and colleagues (2006) found that a school-based vaccination program significantly reduced influenza symptoms in the entire school. School-based mental health services also have been shown to be effective in addressing a wide range of emotional and behavioral issues ( Rones and Hoagwood, 2000 ). School-based health centers have been shown to reduce nonfinancial barriers to health care ( Keyl et al., 1996 ), and families also report more satisfaction with their care than in community or hospital settings ( Kaplan et al., 1999 ).

Conclusion About Health, Nutrition, and Early Learning Safe physical and built environments, health, and nutrition are essential to early learning and academic achievement. Food security and adequate nutrition are important to support cognitive development and participation in education, and food insecurity and poor nutrition can contribute to early learning difficulties. Care and education settings provide an opportunity to promote healthful eating and physical activity in learning environments. Providing appropriate health and developmental screenings and follow-up care and services also is important in supporting development and early learning.

Health and Early Learning: Implications for Adults

Healthy children supported by healthy adults are better prepared to learn. Child health begins prior to conception and extends through pregnancy and throughout childhood. Therefore, the early care and education workforce must be prepared to work across generations to provide education, support, and community linkages to ensure that children grow up poised for success. Ongoing federal support for evidence-based home visiting programs for high-risk families that begin early in pregnancy and continue through early childhood is essential. Professionals working in family childcare, early childhood education centers, preschools, and early elementary schools need to have working knowledge of the relationship between health and children's learning and development. Guidance related to nutrition, physical activity, oral health, immunizations, and preventive health care is essential across all early care and education settings. These professionals also need to be provided with supports and opportunities for close collaboration with health care services and their potential integration into or strengthened linkages with the early care and education setting.

  • EFFECTS OF CHRONIC STRESS AND ADVERSITY

As detailed in Chapter 3 , one of the most important advances in developmental science in recent years has been the recognition that the brain incorporates experience into its development. Although experience is important at any age, early experiences are especially formative in the development of the brain's structure and function. Human development is the result of the continuous interaction of genetics and experience. This interplay is true not just of brain development but of other aspects of human development as well. Research in this area encourages developmental scientists as well as parents and practitioners to consider how positive early experiences and enrichment, in formal and informal ways, may have a beneficial influence on the developing brain and in turn on the growth of thinking and learning. The brain's openness to experience is, however, a double-edged sword—adverse early experiences can have potentially significant negative consequences for brain development and early learning.

As discussed in Chapter 3 , evidence indicates that experiences of stress and adversity are biologically embedded and that individual differences exist in the health and developmental consequences of stress. A substantial body of evidence now shows that adversity and stress in early life are associated with higher rates of childhood mental and physical morbidities, more frequent disturbances in developmental trajectories and educational achievement, and lifelong risks of chronic disorders that compromise health and well- being ( Boyce et al., 2012 ; Hertzman and Boyce, 2010 ; Shonkoff et al., 2009 ). Children respond to stress differently. Many exhibit withdrawal, anger and irritability, difficulty paying attention and concentrating, disturbed sleep, repeated and intrusive thoughts, and extreme distress triggered by things that remind them of their traumatic experiences. Some develop psychiatric conditions such as depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder, and a variety of behavioral disorders ( NCTSN, 2005 ).

What are the circumstances that contribute to chronic adversity and stress for children? All children can experience forms of chronic stress and adversity, but exposures to stress and adversity are socioeconomically layered. Poverty, discussed in more detail below, has been the best studied and is a highly prevalent source of early chronic stress ( Blair and Raver, 2012 ; Evans and Kim, 2013 ; Jiang et al., 2014 ). Young children in the United States also suffer high levels of victimization through child abuse and exposure to domestic violence. The U.S. Department of Health and Human Services reported for the year 2012 that of all child abuse victims, approximately 60 percent were age 8 or younger ( Children's Bureau, 2013 ). The highest rates of child abuse and neglect, including fatalities related to child abuse, were reported for children in the first year of life. Comparable biological and behavioral effects of chronic stress have been studied in children in foster care, in those who experience significant or prolonged family conflict, in those who have a depressed parent, and in those who are abused or neglected (see Thompson, 2014 , for a review).

It is noteworthy that these circumstances include not only those that most people would regard as sources of extreme stress for children (e.g., child abuse), but also those that an adult might regard as less significant because they may be less severe although persistent (e.g., parents' chronic marital conflict, poverty). This broader range of circumstances that children experience as stressful is consistent with the view that, in addition to situations that are manifestly threatening and dangerous, children are stressed by the denial or withdrawal of supportive care, especially when they are young.

Culture also is closely interrelated with stress and adversity. Culture affects the meaning that a child or a family attributes to specific types of traumatic events as well as the ways in which they respond. Because culture also influences expectations regarding the self, others, and social institutions, it can also influence how children and families experience and express distress, grieve or mourn losses, provide support to each other, seek help, and disclose personal information to others. Historical or multigenerational trauma also can influence cultural differences in responses to trauma and loss ( NCTSN Core Curriculum on Childhood Trauma Task Force, 2012 ).

Building on the discussion in Chapter 3 of the biology of chronic stress and adversity, the following sections describe more broadly some of the contributing circumstances and consequences for young children, including the stressors associated with economic adversity; social buffering of stress; and the relationships among stress, learning, and mental health.

The Stressors of Economic Adversity

Children in any economic circumstances can experience stress and adversity, but considerable research on the effects of chronic stress on children's development has focused on children living in families in poverty or with low incomes. The number of children in these conditions of economic adversity is considerable. In 2012, nearly half the children under age 6 lived in poverty or low-income families (defined as up to 200 percent of the federal poverty level, 2 which remains a meagre subsistence) ( Jiang et al., 2014 ). During that same year, more than half the children living with their families in homeless shelters were under the age of 6 ( Child Trends, 2015 ).

The research is clear that poverty as a form of early chronic adversity is a risk factor to long-term physical and mental health, and that for children it can be a significant threat to their capacities to cope with stress, socialize constructively with others, and benefit from the cognitive stimulating opportunities of an early childhood classroom. Socioeconomic disparities in children's experiences of socioemotional adversity and challenging physical environments are well documented (see, e.g., Evans et al., 2012 ). Factors other than economic status itself contribute to the challenges and stresses for children living in low-income families ( Fernald et al., 2013 ). Poverty often is accompanied by the confluence of multiple sources of chronic stress, such as food insufficiency, housing instability (and sometimes homelessness), exposure to violence, environmental noise and toxins, dangerous neighborhoods, poor childcare and schools, family chaos, parents with limited capacity (e.g., resources, education, knowledge/information, time, physical or mental energy) to be supportive and nurturing, parents who are anxious or depressed, parents who are harsh or abusive caregivers, impoverished parent–child communication, and home environments lacking cognitively stimulating activities ( Evans et al., 2012 ; Fernald et al., 2013 ).

As discussed in detail in Chapter 3 , the perturbed biological processes that often accompany economic adversity include changes in the structure and function of children's brain circuitry and dysregulation of their central stress response systems. For these children, therefore, the effects of the chronic stresses associated with economic adversity are likely to contribute to academic, social, and behavioral problems. These problems affect not only early learning and the development of cognitive skills (with impacts on the development of language being best documented) but also the development of learning skills associated with self-regulation and persistence, as well as coping ability, health, and emotional well-being ( Blair and Raver, 2012 ; Evans and Kim, 2013 ).

In addition, developmental consequences related to socioeconomic status are not seen exclusively in children from severely impoverished families. Rather, evidence shows a graded effect of deprivation and adversity across the entire spectrum of socioeconomic status, with even those children from the second-highest social class showing poorer health and development compared with those from families of the very highest socioeconomic status ( Adler et al., 1994 ; Hertzman and Boyce, 2010 ). Moreover, as discussed in Chapter 3 , children are not equally affected by early adverse experiences. Genetic and epigenetic influences may have a role in whether some children are more resilient to early adversity than others ( Rutter, 2012 ).

Detrimental prenatal influences may also be important ( Farah et al., 2008 ; Hackman et al., 2010 ). Although this report focuses on children beginning at birth, child development and early learning also are affected by what a child is exposed to before birth, including influences of family disadvantage. Box 4-6 highlights major research findings on the relationships among family disadvantage, fetal health, and child development.

Family Disadvantage, Fetal Health, and Child Development. Children from different family backgrounds—affected by systemic inequities and disadvantage—start life with starkly different health endowments. As but one example, having a low-birth-weight (more...)

Social Buffering of Stress

The neuroscience of stress has yielded greater understanding of how the effects of stress may be buffered through social support. In behavioral and neurobiological studies of humans and animals, researchers have shown how individuals in adversity show diminished behavioral reactivity and better-regulated cortisol response, among other effects, in the company of people who provide them with emotional support. For children, these individuals often are attachment figures in the family or outside the home.

In health psychology, the benefits of social support for the development and maintenance of healthy practices and the control of disease pathology and healing have been studied since the 1970s (e.g., Cassel, 1976 ; Cobb, 1976 ). Social support also has been recognized as a contributor to psychological well-being for children and youth in difficult circumstances ( Thompson and Goodvin, in press) . In recent years, research on the neurobiology of the social buffering of stress has contributed to a better understanding of why social support has these benefits ( Hostinar et al., 2014 ). In human and animal studies, social companionship in the context of adversity appears to have effects on the biological regulators of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) activity, contributing to greater regulation of stress reactivity through cortical and limbic influences. Social support also appears to stimulate the down-regulation of the proinflammatory tendencies induced by chronic stress, as well as processes driven by neurohormones, including oxytocin, that have other positive benefits ( Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2010 ). Stated differently, social support not only counters the negative effects of chronic stress reactivity but also stimulates constructive influences that contribute independently to greater self-regulation and well-being ( Hostinar et al., 2014 ). This research is still at an early stage, and establishing reliable associations between brain and behavioral functioning in this area is a work in progress, but research findings are providing increasing support for these processes. In one study, for example, greater maternal support measured when children were preschoolers predicted children's larger hippocampus volume at school age ( Luby et al., 2012 ).

The potential benefits of social support as a buffer of chronic stress reactivity underscore the plasticity of developing behavioral and biological systems. Children in adversity need not suffer long-term harms arising from the effects of chronic stress exposure. In a study of families living in rural poverty, for example, toddlers' chronic exposure to domestic violence was associated with elevated cortisol reactivity. However, this effect was buffered when mothers were observed to respond sensitively to their children ( Hibel et al., 2011 ). Experimental interventions designed to change stressful circumstances and promote positive relationships have yielded similar findings. For example a program aimed at easing young children's transition to new foster care placements and promoting warm, responsive, and consistent relationships with new foster parents provided individualized sessions with child therapists, weekly playgroup sessions, and support for foster parents. This program resulted in a normalization of the children's HPA hyporesponsiveness (an effect of stress discussed in Chapter 3 ) ( Fisher et al., 2007 , 2011 ). Another example comes from an intervention based on attachment theory, which trained caregivers to better interpret and respond affectionately to infants and toddlers in foster care and similarly resulted in a normalization of HPA activity and lower cortisol reactivity ( Dozier et al., 2006 , 2008 ). There may be limits to these potential ameliorative effects, depending on the severity and duration of the exposure to adversity. Children who lived for an extended period in profoundly depriving Romanian orphanages, for example, did not show recovery of dysregulated cortisol reactivity, even after a prolonged period of supportive adoptive care ( Gunnar et al., 2001 ).

Because interventions that can help children recover from the effects of chronic adversity can be expensive and time-consuming, however, it appears sensible to try to prevent these effects from occurring. This can be accomplished by reducing exposure to influences that cause significant stress for children, and by strengthening supportive relationships that can buffer its effects. The development of warm, secure attachments between parents and children illustrates the latter approach. As discussed earlier in this chapter, attachment theorists argue that the reliable support provided by a secure attachment relationship enables infants and children to explore and learn from their experiences confidently with the assurance that a trusted adult is available to assist if difficulty ensues. In this view, secure attachments buffer stress and significantly reduce the child's need to be vigilant for threat or danger. As noted previously, attachment research documents a range of benefits associated with secure parent–child relationships in childhood, including greater language skill, academic achievement, and social competence (see Thompson, 2008 , for a review; West et al., 2013 ). The view that these accomplishments are explained, at least in part, by how secure attachments buffer stress for children is supported by studies documenting the better-regulated cortisol reactivity of young children with secure attachments in challenging situations (see Gunnar and Donzella, 2002 , for a review; Nachmias et al., 1996 ).

Viewed in this light, it appears that the contributions adults make to children's learning extend significantly beyond their reading, conversing, counting, and providing other direct forms of cognitive stimulation. An essential contribution is the safety and security they provide that not only buffers children against significant stress when this occurs, but also enables children to invest themselves in learning opportunities with confidence that an adult will assist them when needed. Such confidence not only enables children to learn more from the opportunities afforded them in the family and outside the home but also fosters their developing self-confidence, curiosity, and other learning skills that emerge in the context of secure relationships ( Thompson, 2008 ). This is a benefit of secure, warm adult–child relationships for all children, not just those in adverse circumstances. This phenomenon is perhaps analogous to that seen in studies in which rat pups with nurturant mothers show enhanced learning and memory in low-stress contexts, whereas pups with nonnurturant mothers show greater proficiency in fear conditioning ( Champagne et al., 2008 ).

One problem, however, is that children in adverse circumstances usually have parents and other caregivers who are affected by the same conditions of adversity. Thus, their parents may not be able to provide them with the support they need. This realization has led to the growth of two-generation interventions that are designed to assist children by providing support to their parents in difficult circumstances ( Chase-Lansdale and Brooks-Gunn, 2014 ).

Stress, Learning, and Mental Health

Children learn readily in contexts of social support and emotional well-being, which derive from positive relationships with those who care for and educate them in the family and outside the home. In these contexts, adults can support and encourage developing competencies, convey positive values about learning and school, and help instill curiosity and self-confidence in children. By contrast, learning and cognitive achievement are hindered when children are troubled. This is the case for children from infancy through adolescence who are living in homes with significant marital conflict, when mothers are chronically depressed, when parents are hostile and coercive, or in other circumstances of family turmoil (e.g., Bascoe et al., 2009 ; Brennan et al., 2013 ; Canadian Paediatric Society, 2004 ; Davies et al., 2008 ).

Socioemotional hindrances to learning and cognitive achievement are apparent very early, before children have begun school, and continue to be important as children move into the primary grades. In educational settings, the emotional effects of problems in educator–child relationships can undermine children's performance and their academic success ( Hamre and Pianta, 2004 ; Jeon et al., 2014 ; Pianta, 1999 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ; Skinner and Belmont, 1993 ). As discussed in Chapter 3 , when children are in circumstances of chronic or overwhelming stress, stress hormones affect multiple brain regions, including those relevant to learning, attention, memory, and self-regulation ( McEwen, 2012 ; Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009 ). Over time and with continued exposure to stressful circumstances, these neurocognitive processes become altered as a result of the progressive wear and tear of stress hormones on biological systems as they adapt to this chronic stress. As a consequence, immunologic capacities become weakened (contributing to more frequent acute and chronic illness), self-regulation is impaired (contributing to poorer emotion regulation and impulse control), and cognitive and attentional capabilities are blunted ( Danese and McEwen, 2012 ; Lupien et al., 2009 ; Miller et al., 2011 ). For children, these effects can help account for problems in following instructions, paying attention, managing impulsivity, focusing thinking, and controlling emotions in social encounters—each of which can impair classroom performance and academic achievement.

Young children's vulnerability to stress and their reliance on the support of adults are two central considerations in understanding the foundations for childhood mental health ( IOM and NRC, 2009 ). This relationship among stress, early development, and mental health is relevant to understanding the influences that can threaten the socioemotional well-being of younger children—and to understanding why behavior problems can undermine learning and cognitive growth. One illustration of these effects is the high rates of preschool and prekindergarten children being expelled from their classrooms because of disruptive behavior problems—by one report at a rate more than three times the rate of children in the K-12 grades ( Gilliam, 2005 ; see also U.S. Department of Education Office for Civil Rights, 2014 ). In this study, the likelihood of expulsion decreased significantly when educators were provided access to early childhood mental health consultants who could assist them in managing behavior problems.

Another illustration is reports by kindergarten teachers that social, emotional, and self-regulatory problems are a common impediment to children's readiness to achieve in their classrooms ( Lewit and Baker, 1995 ; Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2000 ). Other studies have shown that children's conduct problems and internalizing (anxious, depressed) behavior in the classroom can undermine the development of constructive educator–child relationships and foreshadow later social and academic difficulties ( Berry and O'Connor, 2010 ; Koles et al., 2009 ; Ladd and Burgess, 2001 ).

Consistent with the research concerning the biological and behavioral effects of chronic stress, there is increasing evidence that even very young children show clear evidence of traumatization and posttraumatic stress, anxious and depressive symptomatology, behavioral and conduct problems, and other serious psychological problems ( Egger and Angold, 2006 ; Lieberman et al., 2011 ; Luby, 2006 ; Zeanah, 2009 ). Sometimes these symptom patterns overlap, such as in the comorbidity in which depressive symptomatology appears along with oppositional behavior in preschoolers ( Egger and Angold, 2006 ). The origins of these problems are multifaceted, but certainly include interaction of environmental stresses with genetic factors that heighten or reduce children's vulnerability to these stresses. Often these environmental stresses undermine the social support that would otherwise buffer the effects of stress on children. Diagnosing these disorders in young children is a challenge because the behaviors associated with early mental health problems in young children can be different from those observed in adults and adolescents ( Egger and Emde, 2011 ). But progress has been made in developing reliable diagnostic criteria for preschoolers (e.g., Egger and Angold, 2006 ; Keenan et al., 1997 ; Lavigne et al., 2009 ) and even infants and toddlers ( Zero to Three, 2005 ). This work provides a foundation for further study of the developmental origins of early mental health challenges and therapeutic interventions that might help these children.

Connecting the Socioemotional Health of Children and Adults

The preceding discussion makes clear that children's socioemotional health is linked to the socioemotional well-being of the adults in their lives. Consistent with the research on the social buffering of stress discussed earlier, when parents and other caregivers are managing well, they can help children cope more competently with the ordinary stresses that inevitably occur. When caregivers are stressed, by contrast, they cannot provide this buffering and are instead more often a source of stress for children. When parents are depressed, for example, they can be unpredictably sad, hostile, critical, and/or disengaged ( NRC and IOM, 2009 ). This constellation of behaviors constitutes a difficult combination of threat and withdrawal of support for children. Young children with a depressed mother are more likely, therefore, to exhibit heightened stress reactivity to moderate challenges; to have an insecure attachment to the parent; to show lower levels of cognitive performance and, later, poorer academic achievement; and to be at greater risk of becoming depressed themselves.

The adult's emotional well-being is important in the classroom as well. Using data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing study, Jeon and colleagues (2014) measured the depressive symptomatology of 761 home- and center-based care providers, as well as overall observed classroom quality, and obtained independent measures of the behavior problems of the 3-year-olds in their classrooms. They found that educator depression was linked to higher levels of behavior problems in children, attributable to the poorer quality of the classroom environment. Notably, this study was conducted with a sample of families in economic stress, with the educators often sharing the same financial difficulties. Nevertheless, the association of educator depression with child behavior problems remained even when family influences, including maternal depression and family poverty status, were controlled for. Similar associations of educator well-being with the quality of the classroom environment and children's learning have been found in studies of children in the early primary grades (e.g., Pianta, 1999 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ).

Conclusions About Chronic Stress and Adversity Chronic stress and adversity constitute fundamental risks to learning and academic success as well as to emotional well-being for many young children. The biological and behavioral effects of stress and adversity can disrupt brain circuitry and stress response systems, affect fundamental cognitive skills, undermine focused thinking and attention, diminish self-regulation, and imperil mental and physical health. Trauma, adversity, and chronic stress can arise from many sources, such as poverty, family conflict, parental depression, abuse, neglect, or exposure to violence in the community. Supportive and stable relationships with adults can help develop children's adaptive capacities and provide them with a significant stress buffer. It is important for adults who work with children to recognize and appreciate the effects of adversity and to have the capacity to employ strategies for preventing or mitigating them, as well as for promoting cognitive, social, and emotional strengths for coping with adverse and stressful experiences. Given the importance of stable and responsive relationships that provide consistent and nurturing interactions, the well-being of the adults who care for young children contributes to their healthy development and early learning. The stresses of economic disadvantage are manifested not only in differences in children's early experiences in the family and the community but also in the quality and stability of the out-of-home care and education families can access and afford and the quality of the schools children later attend. Socioeconomic differences in the quality of early learning opportunities place large numbers of children at a learning disadvantage and undermine their potential for academic success. These differences begin early and have a cumulative effect over time. Strengthening early learning and developing competencies requires serious and sustained attention to these socioeconomic disparities in opportunity.
  • AAP (American Academy of Pediattics) Committee on School Health. Soft drinks in schools. Pediatrics. 2004; 113 (1 Pt. 1):152–154. [ PubMed : 14702469 ]
  • AAP Committee on Sports Medicine and Fitness and AAP Committee on School Health. Physical fitness and activity in schools. Pediatrics. 2000; 105 (5):1156–1157. [ PubMed : 10790480 ]
  • AAP Council on Early Childhood. High PC, Klass P. Literacy promotion: An essential component of primary care pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2014; 134 (2):404–409. [ PubMed : 24962987 ]
  • AAP Council on School Health. The crucial role of recess in school. Pediatrics. 2013; 131 (1):183–188. [ PubMed : 23277311 ]
  • Abu-Saad K, Fraser D. Maternal nutrition and birth outcomes. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2010; 32 (1):5–25. [ PubMed : 20237078 ]
  • Adams MJ. Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990.
  • Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, Syme SL. Socioeconomic status and health. The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist. 1994; 49 (1):15–24. [ PubMed : 8122813 ]
  • Ahnert L, Pinquart M, Lamb ME. Security of children's relationships with nonparental care providers: A meta-analysis. Child Development. 2006; 77 (3):664–679. [ PubMed : 16686794 ]
  • Aizer A. Poverty, violence, and health: The impact of domestic violence during pregnancy on newborn health. Journal of Human Resources. 2011; 46 (3):518–538. [ PMC free article : PMC4019993 ] [ PubMed : 24839303 ]
  • Aizer A, Currie J. The intergenerational transmission of inequality: Maternal disadvantage and health at birth. Science. 2014; 344 (6186):856–861. [ PMC free article : PMC4578153 ] [ PubMed : 24855261 ]
  • Alaimo K, Olson CM, Frongillo EA Jr. Food insufficiency and American school-aged children's cognitive, academic, and psychosocial development. Pediatrics. 2001; 108 (1):44–53. [ PubMed : 11433053 ]
  • Alexander KL, Entwisle DR, Dauber SL. First-grade classroom behavior: Its short- and long-term consequences for school performance. Child Development. 1993; 64 (3):801–814. [ PubMed : 8339696 ]
  • Almond D, Currie J. Killing me softly: The fetal origins hypothesis. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2011; 25 (3):153–172. [ PMC free article : PMC4140221 ] [ PubMed : 25152565 ]
  • Almond D, Mazumder B. Health capital and the prenatal environment: The effect of Ramadan observance during pregnancy. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2011; 3 (4):56–85.
  • Aloise-Young PA. The development of self-presentation: Self-promotion in 6- to 10-year-old children. Social Cognition. 1993; 11 (2):201–222.
  • Anghel D. Executive function in preschool children: Working memory as a predictor of mathematical ability at school age. Revista Romaneasca pentru Educatie Multidimensionala. 2010; 2 (4):5–16.
  • Ansari D, Garcia N, Lucas E, Hamon K, Dhital B. Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults. Neuroreport. 2005; 16 :1769–1775. [ PubMed : 16237324 ]
  • Antell SE, Keating DP. Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. Child Development. 1983; 54 :695–701. [ PubMed : 6851716 ]
  • Anthony JL, Lonigan CJ, Driscoll K, Phillips BM, Burgess SR. Phonological sensitivity: A quasi-parallel progression of word structure units and cognitive operations. Reading Research Quarterly. 2003; 38 (4):470–487.
  • Ashcraft MH. Math performance, working memory, and math anxiety; some possible directions for neural functioning work; Paper read at The Neural Basis of Mathematical Development; November; Nashville, TN. 2006.
  • Ashkenazi S, Mark-Zigdon N, Henik A. Do subitizing deficits in developmental dyscalculia involve pattern recognition weakness? Developmental Science. 2013; 16 (1):35–46. [ PubMed : 23278925 ]
  • Au TK, Chan CK, Chan TK, Cheung MW, Ho JY, Ip GW. Folkbiology meets microbiology: A study of conceptual and behavioral change. Cognitive Psychology. 2008; 57 (1):1–19. [ PubMed : 18457822 ]
  • Aukrust VG. Young children acquiring second language vocabulary in preschool group-time: Does amount, diversity, and discourse complexity of teacher talk matter? Journal of Research in Childhood Education. 2007; 22 (1):17–37.
  • Aunola K, Leskinen E, Lerkkanen M-K, Nurmi J-E. Developmental dynamics of math performance from pre-school to grade 2. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004; 96 :699–713.
  • Baillargeon R, Wu D, Yuan S, Li J, Luo Y. Young infants' expectations about self-propelled objects. In: Hood BM, Santos L, editors. The origins of object knowledge. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2009.
  • Baillargeon R, Scott RM, He Z. False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010; 14 (3):110–118. [ PMC free article : PMC2930901 ] [ PubMed : 20106714 ]
  • Baker D, Knipe H, Collins J, Leon J, Cummings E, Blair C, Gramson D. One hundred years of elementary school mathematics in the United States: A content analysis and cognitive assessment of textbooks from 1900 to 2000. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 2010; 41 (4):383–423.
  • Baldwin DA. Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development. 1991; 62 (5):875–890. [ PubMed : 1756664 ]
  • Baldwin DA, Moses LJ. Links between social understanding and early word learning: Challenges to current accounts. Social Development. 2001; 10 (3):309–329.
  • Baldwin DA, Tomasello M. Word learning: A window on early pragmatic understanding. Clark EV, editor. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; The Proceedings of the Twenty-ninth Annual Child Language Research Forum. 1998; 29 :3–24.
  • Ball EW, Blachman BA. Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly. 1991; 26 (1):49–66.
  • Barata MC. Executive functions in Chilean preschool children: Investigating the associations of early executive functions with emergent mathematics and literacy skills. Harvard Graduate School of Education; 2010. (PhD diss.).
  • Barkley RA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: Toward a more comprehensive theory. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1997; 18 (4):271–279. [ PubMed : 9276836 ]
  • Baroody AE, Dobbs-Oates J. Child and parent characteristics, parental expectations, and child behaviours related to preschool children's interest in literacy. Early Child Development and Care. 2009; 181 (3):345–359.
  • Baroody AJ. The developmental bases for early childhood number and operations standards. In: Clements DH, Sarama J, DiBiase A-M, editors. Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 173–219.
  • Baroody AJ, Lai M-L, Mix KS. Changing views of young children's numerical and arithmetic competencies; Paper read at National Association for the Education of Young Children; December; Washington, DC. 2005.
  • Baroody AJ, Lai M-L, Mix KS. The development of young children's number and operation sense and its implications for early childhood education. In: Spodek B, Saracho ON, editors. Handbook of research on the education of young children. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006. pp. 187–221.
  • Baroody AJ, Li X, Lai M-l. Toddlers' spontaneous attention to number. Mathematical Thinking and Learning. 2008; 10 :240–270.
  • Bartsch K, Wellman HM. Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995.
  • Basch CE. Breakfast and the achievement gap among urban minority youth. Journal of School Health. 2011; 81 (10):635–640. [ PubMed : 21923876 ]
  • Bascoe SM, Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Cummings EM. Children's representations of family relationships, peer information processing, and school adjustment. Developmental Psychology. 2009; 45 (6):1740–1751. [ PMC free article : PMC2912155 ] [ PubMed : 19899928 ]
  • Bassett HH, Denham S, Wyatt TM, Warren-Khot HK. Refining the preschool self-regulation assessment for use in preschool classrooms. Infant and Child Development. 2012; 21 (6):596–616.
  • Bassok D, Rorem A. Is kindergarten the new first grade? The changing nature of kindergarten in the age of accountability. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia; 2014.
  • Beilock SL. Learning and performing math: Self-concept, self-doubt, and self-fulfilling prophesy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001; 130 :224–237. [ PubMed : 11409101 ]
  • Belsky DW, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Melchior M, Caspi A. Context and sequelae of food insecurity in children's development. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2010; 172 (7):809–818. [ PMC free article : PMC2984258 ] [ PubMed : 20716700 ]
  • Bennett N, Desforges C, Cockburn A, Wilkinson B. The quality of pupil learning experiences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1984.
  • Berch DB, Mazzocco MMM, editors. Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 2007.
  • Berry D, O'Connor E. Behavioral risk, teacher–child relationships, and social skill development across middle childhood: A child-by-environment analysis of change. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2010; 31 (1):1–14.
  • Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences. 2011; 21 (4):327–336. [ PMC free article : PMC3155246 ] [ PubMed : 21845021 ]
  • Bharadwaj P, Johnsen JV, Løken KV. Smoking bans, maternal smoking and birth outcomes. 2012. (IZA Institute for the Study of Labor Discussion Paper No. 7006:72-93).
  • Bharadwaj P, Eberhard J, Neilson C. Health at birth, parental investments and academic outcomes. San Diego: University of California, San Diego; 2013. (unpublished)
  • Bialystok E. Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2011; 65 (4):229–235. [ PMC free article : PMC4341987 ] [ PubMed : 21910523 ]
  • Bialystok E, Craik FIM. Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010; 19 (1):19–23.
  • Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Green DW, Gollan TH. Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2009; 10 (3):89–129. [ PubMed : 26168404 ]
  • Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Doyle AE, Seidman LJ, Wilens TE, Ferrero F, Morgan CL, Faraone SV. Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004; 72 (5):757–766. [ PubMed : 15482034 ]
  • Bielaczyc K, Pirolli PL, Brown AL. Training in self-explanation and self-regulation strategies: Investigating the effects of knowledge acquisition activities on problem solving. Cognition and Instruction. 1995; 13 :221–252.
  • Bierman KL, Nix RL, Greenberg MT, Blair C, Domitrovich CE. Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology. 2008a; 20 (3):821–843. [ PMC free article : PMC3205459 ] [ PubMed : 18606033 ]
  • Bierman KL, Domitrovich CE, Nix RL, Gest SD, Welsh JA, Greenberg MT, Blair C, Nelson KE, Gill S. Promoting academic and social-emotional school readiness: The Head Start REDI program. Child Development. 2008b; 79 (6):1802–1817. [ PMC free article : PMC3549580 ] [ PubMed : 19037951 ]
  • Birch SH, Ladd GW. The teacher–child relationship and children's early school adjustment. Journal of School Psychology. 1997; 35 (1):61–79.
  • Birch SH, Ladd GW. Children's interpersonal behaviors and the teacher–child relationship. Developmental Psychology. 1998; 34 (5):934–946. [ PubMed : 9779740 ]
  • Blair C. School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American Psychologist. 2002; 57 (2):111–127. [ PubMed : 11899554 ]
  • Blair C, Diamond A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology. 2008; 20 (3):899–911. [ PMC free article : PMC2593474 ] [ PubMed : 18606037 ]
  • Blair C, Raver CC. Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist. 2012; 67 (4):309–318. [ PMC free article : PMC5264526 ] [ PubMed : 22390355 ]
  • Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development. 2007; 78 :647–663. [ PubMed : 17381795 ]
  • Blair C, Protzko J, Ursache A. Self-regulation and early literacy. Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. New York: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2010; 3 :20–35.
  • Blevins-Knabe B, Musun-Miller L. Number use at home by children and their parents and its relationship to early mathematical performance. Early Development and Parenting. 1996; 5 :35–45.
  • Bloom P. Just babies: The origins of good and evil. New York: Crown Publishers; 2013.
  • Bodovski K, Farkas G. Mathematics growth in early elementary school: The roles of beginning knowledge, student engagement, and instruction. Elementary School Journal. 2007; 108 (2):115–130.
  • Bodrova E, Leong DJ. Scaffolding self-regulated learning in young children: Lessons from tools of the mind. In: Pianta RC, Barnett WS, Justice LM, Sheridan SM, editors. Handbook of early childhood education. New York: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 352–369.
  • Bougma K, Aboud FE, Harding KB, Marquis GS. Iodine and mental development of children 5 years old and under: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. 2013; 5 (4):1384–1416. [ PMC free article : PMC3705354 ] [ PubMed : 23609774 ]
  • Bowers EP, Vasilyeva M. The relation between teacher input and lexical growth of preschoolers. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2011; 32 (1):221–241.
  • Boyce WT, Sokolowski MB, Robinson GE. Toward a new biology of social adversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (Suppl. 2):17143–17148. [ PMC free article : PMC3477390 ] [ PubMed : 23045689 ]
  • Bradley L, Bryant PE. Categorizing sounds and learning to read—a causal connection. Nature. 1983; 301 (5899):419–421.
  • Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002; 53 :371–399. [ PubMed : 11752490 ]
  • Brennan LM, Shelleby EC, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Dishion TJ, Wilson M. Indirect effects of the family check-up on school-age academic achievement through improvements in parenting in early childhood. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013; 105 (3) [ PMC free article : PMC3850059 ] [ PubMed : 24319295 ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Broidy LM, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, Brame B, Dodge KA, Fergusson D, Horwood J, Loeber R, Laird R, Lyname D, Moffit TF, Bates JE, Pettit GS, Vitaro F. Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology. 2003; 30 (2):222–245. [ PMC free article : PMC2753823 ] [ PubMed : 12661883 ]
  • Brosnan M, Demetre J, Hamill S, Robson K, Shepherd H, Cody G. Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2002; 40 (12):2144–2155. [ PubMed : 12208010 ]
  • Bruner J. The role of dialogue in language acquisition. In: Sinclair A, Jarvella R, Levelt WJM, editors. The child's conception of language. New York: Springer; 1978. pp. 241–256.
  • Bryan J, Osendarp S, Hughes D, Calvaresi E, Baghurst K, van Klinken JW. Nutrients for cognitive development in school-aged children. Nutrition Reviews. 2004; 62 (8):295–306. [ PubMed : 15478684 ]
  • Bühler E, Bachmann C, Goyert H, Heinzel-Gutenbrunner M, Kamp-Becker I. Differential diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder by means of inhibitory control and “theory of mind.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011; 41 :1718–1726. [ PubMed : 21373957 ]
  • Buhs ES, Ladd GW. Peer rejection as an antecedent of young children's school adjustment: An examination of mediating processes. Developmental Psychology. 2001; 37 (4):550–560. [ PubMed : 11444490 ]
  • Bull R, Scerif G. Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2001; 19 (3):273–293. [ PubMed : 11758669 ]
  • Bull R, Johnston RS, Roy JA. Exploring the roles of the visual-spatial sketch pad and central executive in children's arithmetical skills: Views from cognition and developmental neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1999; 15 (3):421–442.
  • Bull R, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008; 33 :205–228. [ PMC free article : PMC2729141 ] [ PubMed : 18473197 ]
  • Busch-Rossnagel NA. Mastery motivation, preschool and early childhood. In: Fisher C, Lerner R, editors. Encyclopedia of applied developmental science. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.; 2005. pp. 679–681.
  • Butler LP, Markman EM. Finding the cause: Verbal framing helps children extract causal evidence embedded in a complex scene. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2012a; 13 (1):38–66.
  • Butler LP, Markman EM. Preschoolers use intentional and pedagogical cues to guide inductive inferences and exploration. Child Development. 2012b; 83 (4):1416–1428. [ PubMed : 22540939 ]
  • Butler LP, Markman EM. Preschoolers use pedagogical cues to guide radical reorganization of category knowledge. Cognition. 2014; 130 (1):116–127. [ PubMed : 24211439 ]
  • Butterworth B. The development of arithmetical abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005; 46 :3–18. [ PubMed : 15660640 ]
  • Butterworth B. Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010; 14 :534–541. [ PubMed : 20971676 ]
  • Butterworth B, Varma S, Laurillard D. Dyscalculia: From brain to education. Science. 2011; 332 :1049–1053. [ PubMed : 21617068 ]
  • Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1989; 81 (3):313–321.
  • Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 2- and 3-year follow-up and a new preschool trial. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1995; 87 :488–503.
  • Byrnes JP, Wasik BA. Factors predictive of mathematics achievement in kindergarten, first and third grades: An opportunity–propensity analysis. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2009; 34 :167–183.
  • Cameron CE, Brock LL, Murrah WM, Bell LH, Worzalla SL, Grissmer D, Morrison FJ. Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Development. 2012; 83 (4):1229–1244. [ PMC free article : PMC3399936 ] [ PubMed : 22537276 ]
  • Campbell F, Conti G, Heckman JJ, Moon SH, Pinto R, Pungello E, Pan Y. Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science. 2014; 343 (6178):1478–1485. [ PMC free article : PMC4028126 ] [ PubMed : 24675955 ]
  • Canadian Paediatric Society. Maternal depression and child development. Paediatrics & Child Health. 2004; 9 (8):575–583. [ PMC free article : PMC2724169 ] [ PubMed : 19680490 ]
  • Cantlon JF, Brannon EM, Carter EJ, Pelphrey KA. Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4-y-old children. PLoS Biology. 2006; 4 (5):e125. [ PMC free article : PMC1431577 ] [ PubMed : 16594732 ]
  • Carey S. The origin of concepts. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press; 2009.
  • Carneiro P, Ginja R. Long term impacts of compensatory preschool on health and behavior: Evidence from Head Start. Bonn, Germany: Institute for the Study of Labor; 2012. (IZA Discussion Paper No. 6315).
  • Cartwright KB. Cognitive development and reading: The relation of reading-specific multiple classification skill to reading comprehension in elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002; 94 (1):56–63.
  • Cartwright KB. Cognitive flexibility and reading comprehension: Relevance to the future. In: Block CC, Parris SR, editors. Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices. 2nd. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 50–64.
  • Cartwright KB, Marshall TR, Dandy KL, Isaac MC. The development of graphophonological-semantic cognitive flexibility and its contribution to reading comprehension in beginning readers. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2010; 11 (1):61–85.
  • Cassel J. The contribution of the social environment to host resistance: The fourth Wade Hampton Frost lecture. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1976; 104 (2):107–123. [ PubMed : 782233 ]
  • Catts HW, Kamhi AG. Language and reading disabilities. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 1999.
  • Catts HW, Fey ME, Zhang X, Tomblin JB. Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading. 1999; 3 (4):331–361.
  • CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). School health guidelines to promote healthy eating and physical activity. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2011; 60 (RR05):1–71. [ PubMed : 21918496 ]
  • CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2011. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report: Surveillance Summaries. 2012; 61 (4):1–162. [ PubMed : 22673000 ]
  • CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). Middle childhood (6-8 years of age): Developmental milestones. 2014. [September 15, 2014]. http://www ​.cdc.gov/ncbddd ​/childdevelopment ​/positiveparenting/middle.html .
  • Celedón-Pattichis S, Musanti SI, Marshall ME. Bilingual elementary teachers' reflections on using students' native language and culture to teach mathematics. In: Foote MQ, editor. Mathematics teaching & learning in K-12: Equity and professional development. New York: Palgrave Macmillan; 2010. pp. 7–24.
  • Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University. The foundations of lifelong health are built in early childhood. 2010. [January 22, 2015]. http://www ​.developingchild.harvard.edu .
  • Champagne DL, Bagot RC, van Hasselt F, Ramakers G, Meaney MJ, de Kloet ER, Joels M, Krugers H. Maternal care and hippocampal plasticity: Evidence for experience-dependent structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and differential responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008; 28 (23):6037–6045. [ PMC free article : PMC6670331 ] [ PubMed : 18524909 ]
  • Chase-Lansdale L, Brooks-Gunn J. Two-generation programs in the twenty-first century. Future of Children. 2014; 24 (1):13–39. [ PubMed : 25518701 ]
  • Chi MTH, Klahr D. Span and rate of apprehension in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1975; 19 :434–439. [ PubMed : 1236928 ]
  • Child Trends. Homeless children and youth. 2015. [January 27, 2015]. http://www ​.childtrends ​.org/?indicators=homeless-children-and-youth .
  • Children's Bureau. Child maltreatment 2012. 2013. [January 27, 2015]. http://www ​.acf.hhs.gov ​/programs/cb/research-data-technology ​/statistics-research ​/child-maltreatment .
  • Chu FW, Vanmarle K, Geary DC. Quantitative deficits of preschool children at risk for mathematical learning disability. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013; 4 :195. [ PMC free article : PMC3655274 ] [ PubMed : 23720643 ]
  • Cimpian A. The impact of generic language about ability on children's achievement motivation. Developmental Psychology. 2010; 46 (5):1333–1340. [ PubMed : 20822242 ]
  • Cimpian A, Markman EM. Information learned from generic language becomes central to children's biological concepts: Evidence from their open-ended explanations. Cognition. 2009; 113 (1):14–25. [ PubMed : 19674739 ]
  • Cimpian A, Markman EM. The generic/nongeneric distinction influences how children interpret new information about social others. Child Development. 2011; 82 (2):471–492. [ PubMed : 21410911 ]
  • Cimpian A, Arce HC, Markman EM, Dweck CS. Subtle linguistic cues affect children's motivation. Psychological Science. 2007; 18 (4):314–316. [ PubMed : 17470255 ]
  • Claessens A, Engel M, Curran FC. Academic content, student learning, and the persistence of preschool effects. American Educational Research Journal. 2014; 51 (2):403–434.
  • Clarke BA, Clarke DM, Cheeseman J. The mathematical knowledge and understanding young children bring to school. Mathematics Education Research Journal. 2006; 18 (1):81–107.
  • Clements DH. Subitizing: What is it? Why teach it? Teaching Children Mathematics. 1999; 5 :400–405.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning trajectories in mathematics education. Mathematical Thinking & Learning. 2004; 6 (2):81–89.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Experimental evaluation of the effects of a research-based preschool mathematics curriculum. American Educational Research Journal. 2008; 45 :443–494.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning and teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. New York: Routledge; 2009.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning and teaching early and elementary mathematics. In: Carlson JS, Levin JR, editors. Instructional strategies for improving students' learning: Focus on early reading and mathematics. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing; 2012.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning and teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. 2nd. New York: Routledge; 2014.
  • Clements DH, Swaminathan S, Hannibal MAZ, Sarama J. Young children's concepts of shape. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 1999; 30 :192–212.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J, Spitler ME, Lange AA, Wolfe CB. Mathematics learned by young children in an intervention based on learning trajectories: A large-scale cluster randomized trial. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 2011; 42 (2):127–166.
  • Clements DH, Baroody AJ, Sarama J. Background research on early mathematics. National Governor's Association (NGA) Center Project on Early Mathematics; 2013a. (unpublished)
  • Clements DH, Sarama J, Wolfe CB, Spitler ME. Longitudinal evaluation of a scale-up model for teaching mathematics with trajectories and technologies: Persistence of effects in the third year. American Educational Research Journal. 2013b; 50 (4):812–850.
  • Cobb S. Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1976; 38 (5):300–314. [ PubMed : 981490 ]
  • Colman S, Nichols-Barrer IP, Redline JE, Devaney BL, Ansell SV, Joyce T. Effects of the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC): A review of recent research (summary). Alexandria, VA: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service, Office of Research and Analysis; 2012.
  • Connor CM, Morrison FJ, Slominski L. Preschool instruction and children's emergent literacy growth. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006; 98 (4):665–689.
  • Connor CM, Morrison FJ, Underwood PS. A second chance in second grade: The independent and cumulative impact of first- and second-grade reading instruction and students' letter-word reading skill growth. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2007; 11 (3):199–233.
  • Connor CM, Alberto PA, Compton DL, O'Connor RE. Improving reading outcomes for students with or at risk for reading disabilities: A synthesis of the contributions from the Institute of Education Sciences Research Centers. Washington, DC: National Center for Special Education Research; 2014. (NCSER 2014-3000).
  • Conway ARA, Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003; 7 (12):547–552. [ PubMed : 14643371 ]
  • Cook JT, Frank DA. Food security, poverty, and human development in the United States. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008; 1136 (1):193–209. [ PubMed : 17954670 ]
  • Cooper DH, Roth FP, Speece DL, Schatschneider C. The contribution of oral language skills to the development of phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2002; 23 :399–416.
  • Cooper SB, Bandelow S, Nute ML, Morris JG, Nevill ME. Breakfast glycaemic index and cognitive function in adolescent school children. British Journal of Nutrition. 2012; 107 (12):1823–1832. [ PubMed : 22017815 ]
  • Copple C, Bredekamp S, Koralek DG, Charner K. Developmentally appropriate practice. Focus on preschoolers. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children; 2013.
  • Coyne MD, McCoach DB, Loftus S, Zipoli R Jr., Kapp S. Direct vocabulary instruction in kindergarten: Teaching for breadth versus depth. Elementary School Journal. 2009; 110 (1):1–18.
  • Csibra G. Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language. 2010; 25 (2):141–168.
  • Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009; 13 (4):148–153. [ PubMed : 19285912 ]
  • Cunha F, Heckman JJ. Investing in our young people. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2010. (Working paper 1620).
  • Cunningham AE, Zibulsky J. Book smart: How to develop and support successful, motivated readers. New York: Oxford University Press; 2014.
  • Currie J. Inequality at birth: Some causes and consequences. American Economic Review. 2011; 101 (3):1–22.
  • Currie J, Rossin-Slater M. Weathering the storm: Hurricanes and birth outcomes. Journal of Health Economics. 2013; 32 (3):487–503. [ PMC free article : PMC3649867 ] [ PubMed : 23500506 ]
  • Currie J, Walker WR. Traffic congestion and infant health: Evidence from E-ZPass. 2009. (National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series No. 15413).
  • Currie J, Neidell M, Schmieder JF. Air pollution and infant health: Lessons from New Jersey. Journal of Health Economics. 2009; 28 (3):688–703. [ PMC free article : PMC2727943 ] [ PubMed : 19328569 ]
  • Currie J, Graff Zivin J, Meckel K, Neidell M, Schlenker W. Something in the water: Contaminated drinking water and infant health. Canadian Journal of Economics. 2013; 46 (3):791–810. [ PMC free article : PMC4849482 ] [ PubMed : 27134285 ]
  • Danese A, McEwen BS. Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiology & Behavior. 2012; 106 (1):29–39. [ PubMed : 21888923 ]
  • Davies PT, Woitach MJ, Winter MA, Cummings EM. Children's insecure representations of the interparental relationship and their school adjustment: The mediating role of attention difficulties. Child Development. 2008; 79 (5):1570–1582. [ PubMed : 18826543 ]
  • de Bruin A, Treccani B, Della Sala S. Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science. 2015; 26 (1):99–107. [ PubMed : 25475825 ]
  • de Ruiter C, van IJzendoorn MH. International Journal of Educational Research. 19. 1993. Attachment and cognition: A review of the literature; pp. 525–540.
  • Dearing E, McCartney K, Taylor BA. Change in family income-to-needs matters more for children with less. Child Development. 2001; 72 (6):1779–1793. [ PubMed : 11768145 ]
  • Denham SA, Brown C. “Plays nice with others”: Social–emotional learning and academic success. Early Education and Development. 2010; 21 (5):652–680.
  • Denton K, West J. Children's reading and mathematics achievement in kindergarten and first grade. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics; 2002.
  • Derryberry D, Reed M. Regulatory processes and the development of cognitive representations. Development and Psychopathology. 1996; 8 (1):215–234.
  • Deschenes O, Greenstone M, Guryan J. Climate change and birth weight. American Economic Review. 2009; 99 (2):211–217. [ PubMed : 29505213 ]
  • DeYoung CG. Intelligence and personality. In: Sternberg RJ, Kaufman SB, editors. The Cambridge handbook of intelligence. Cambridge, MA, and New York: Cambridge University Press; 2011. pp. 711–737.
  • Diamond A. Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In: Carey SE, Gelman R, editors. The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1991. pp. 67–110.
  • Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011; 333 (6045):959–964. [ PMC free article : PMC3159917 ] [ PubMed : 21852486 ]
  • Diamond KE, Justice LM, Siegler RS, Snyder PA. Synthesis of IES research on early intervention and early childhood education. Washington, DC: National Center for Special Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; 2013.
  • Dickinson DK. Why we must improve teacher–child conversations in preschools and the promise of professional development. In: Girolametto L, Weitzman E, editors. Enhancing caregiver language facilitation in childcare settings. Toronto: The Hanen Institute; 2003. pp. 41–48.
  • Dickinson DK, Freiberg J. Environmental factors affecting language acquisition from birth–five: Implications for literacy development and intervention efforts; Paper presented at Workshop on the Role of Language in School Learning: Implications for Closing the Achievement Gap; Menlo Park, CA. 2009.
  • Dickinson DK, Porche MV. Relation between language experiences in preschool classrooms and children's kindergarten and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Child Development. 2011; 82 (3):870–886. [ PubMed : 21413936 ]
  • Dickinson DK, Smith MW. Preschool talk: Patterns of teacher–child interaction in early childhood classrooms. Journal of Research in Childhood Education. 1991; 6 (1):20–29.
  • Dickinson DK, Tabors PO. Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 2001.
  • Dickinson DK, Darrow CL, Tinubu TA. Patterns of teacher–child conversations in Head Start classrooms: Implications for an empirically grounded approach to professional development. Early Education and Development. 2008; 19 (3):396–429.
  • Doig B, McCrae B, Rowe K. A good start to numeracy: Effective numeracy strategies from research and practice in early childhood. Canberra ACT, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research; 2003.
  • Dombek JL, Connor CM. Preventing retention: First grade classroom instruction and student characteristics. Psychology in the Schools. 2012; 49 (6):568–588.
  • Domitrovich CE, Moore JE, Thompson RA. the CASEL Preschool to Elementary School Social and Emotional Learning Assessment Workgroup. Interventions that promote social-emotional learning in young children. In: Pianta RC, Barnett WS, Justice LM, Sheridan SM, editors. Handbook of early childhood education. New York: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 393–415.
  • Dozier M, Peloso E, Lindhiem O, Gordon MK, Manni M, Sepulveda S, Ackerman J, Bernier A, Levine S. Developing evidence-based interventions for foster children: An example of a randomized clinical trial with infants and toddlers. Journal of Social Issues. 2006; 62 (4):767–785.
  • Dozier M, Peloso E, Lewis E, Laurenceau JP, Levine S. Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Development and Psychopathology. 2008; 20 (3):845–859. [ PMC free article : PMC3258505 ] [ PubMed : 18606034 ]
  • Drake K, Belsky J, Fearon RM. From early attachment to engagement with learning in school: The role of self-regulation and persistence. Developmental Psychology. 2014; 50 (5):1350–1361. [ PubMed : 23647414 ]
  • Duke NK, Block MK. Improving reading in the primary grades. The Future of Children. 2012; 22 (2):55–72. [ PubMed : 23057131 ]
  • Duncan GJ, Magnuson K. The nature and impact of early achievement skills, attention skills, and behavior problems. In: Duncan GJ, Murnane R, editors. Whither opportunity? Rising inequality and the uncertain life chances of low-income children. New York: Russell Sage Press; 2011. pp. 47–70.
  • Duncan GJ, Claessens A, Engel M. The contributions of hard skills and socioemotional behavior to school readiness. 2005. [January 20, 2015]. http://www ​.ipr.northwestern ​.edu/publications ​/docs/workingpapers ​/2005/IPR-WP-05-01.pdf .
  • Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, Klebanov P, Pagani L, Feinstein L, Engel M, Brooks-Gunn J, Sexton H, Duckworth K, Japel C. School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43 (6):1428–1446. [ PubMed : 18020822 ]
  • Dunlap G, Strain PS, Fox L, Carta JJ, Conroy M, Smith BJ, Kern L, Hemmeter ML, Timm MA, McCart A, Sailor W, Markey U, Markey DJ, Lardieri S, Sowell C. Prevention and intervention with young children's challenging behavior: Perspectives regarding current knowledge. Behavioral Disorders. 2006; 32 (1):29–45.
  • Dunn J. Mindreading, emotion understanding, and relationships. In: Hartup WW, Silbereisen RK, editors. Growing points in developmental science: An introduction. Hove, NY: Psychology Press; 2002. pp. 167–176.
  • Edens KM, Potter EF. An exploratory look at the relationships among math skills, motivational factors and activity choice. Early Childhood Education Journal. 2013; 41 (3):235–243.
  • Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2006; 47 (3-4):313–337. [ PubMed : 16492262 ]
  • Egger HL, Emde RN. Developmentally sensitive diagnostic criteria for mental health disorders in early childhood: The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV, the research diagnostic criteria-preschool age, and the diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood-revised. American Psychologist. 2011; 66 (2):95–106. [ PMC free article : PMC3064438 ] [ PubMed : 21142337 ]
  • Ehri LC. Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2005; 9 (2):167–188.
  • Eimeren LV, MacMillan KD, Ansari D. The role of subitizing in children's development of verbal counting; Paper read at Society for Research in Child Development; April; Boston, MA. 2007.
  • Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Eggum ND. Self-regulation and school readiness. Early Education and Development. 2010; 21 (5):681–698. [ PMC free article : PMC3018834 ] [ PubMed : 21234283 ]
  • Ellis R. The study of second language acquisition. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press; 2008.
  • Englund MM, Luckner AE, Whaley GJL, Egeland B. Children's achievement in early elementary school: Longitudinal effects of parental involvement, expectations, and quality of assistance. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004; 96 (4):723–730.
  • Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Tesch-Römer C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review. 1993; 100 :363–406.
  • ERS (Economic Research Service). Definitions of food security. 2014. [March 23, 2015]. http://www ​.ers.usda.gov ​/topics/food-nutrition-assistance ​/food-security-in-the-us ​/definitions-of-food-security.aspx .
  • Espada JP. The native language in teaching kindergarten mathematics. Journal of International Education Research. 2012; 8 (4):359–366.
  • Espinosa LM. Curriculum and assessment considerations for young children from culturally, linguistically, and economically diverse backgrounds. Psychology in the Schools. 2005; 42 (8):837–853.
  • Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives. 2013; 7 (1):43–48.
  • Evans GW, Chen E, Miller G, Seeman T. How poverty gets under the skin: A life-course perspective. In: Maholmes V, King RB, editors. The Oxford handbook of poverty and child development. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 13–36.
  • Fahie CM, Symons DK. Executive functioning and theory of mind in children clinically referred for attention and behavior problems. Applied Developmental Psychology. 2003; 24 :51–73.
  • Farah MJ, Betancourt L, Shera DM, Savage JH, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Malmud EK, Hurt H. Environmental stimulation, parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Developmental Science. 2008; 11 (5):793–801. [ PubMed : 18810850 ]
  • Farran DC, Aydogan C, Kang SJ, Lipsey M. Preschool classroom environments and the quantity and quality of children's literacy and language behaviors. In: Dickinson D, Neuman S, editors. Handbook of early literacy research. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. pp. 257–268.
  • Farran DC, Lipsey MW, Watson B, Hurley S. Balance of content emphasis and child content engagement in an early reading first program; Paper presented at American Educational Research Association; April; Chicago, IL. 2007.
  • Feigenson L, Libertus ME, Halberda J. Links between the intuitive sense of number and formal mathematics ability. Child Development Perspectives. 2013; 7 (2):74–79. [ PMC free article : PMC3891767 ] [ PubMed : 24443651 ]
  • Fernald A, Marchman VA, Weisleder A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science. 2013; 16 (2):234–248. [ PMC free article : PMC3582035 ] [ PubMed : 23432833 ]
  • Field E, Robles O, Torero M. Iodine deficiency and schooling attainment in Tanzania. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2009; 1 (4):140–169.
  • Figlio D, Guryan J, Karbownik K, Roth J. The effects of poor neonatal health on children's cognitive development? American Economic Review. 2014; 104 (12):4205–4230. [ PubMed : 29533575 ]
  • Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M, Gunnar MR, Burraston BO. Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007; 32 (8-10):892–905. [ PMC free article : PMC2174427 ] [ PubMed : 17656028 ]
  • Fisher PA, Van Ryzin MJ, Gunnar MR. Mitigating HPA axis dysregulation associated with placement changes in foster care. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011; 36 (4):531–539. [ PMC free article : PMC3610565 ] [ PubMed : 20888698 ]
  • Fivush R, Haden CA, Reese E. Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development. 2006; 77 (6):1568–1588. [ PubMed : 17107447 ]
  • Florence MD, Asbridge M, Veugelers PJ. Diet quality and academic performance. (quiz 239-241). Journal of School Health. 2008; 78 (4):209–215. [ PubMed : 18336680 ]
  • Furrow D, Nelson K, Benedict H. Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language. 1979; 6 (03):423–442. [ PubMed : 536408 ]
  • Fuson KC. Children's counting and concepts of number. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1988.
  • Fuson KC. Research on learning and teaching addition and subtraction of whole numbers. In: Leinhardt G, Putman R, Hattrup RA, editors. Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992. pp. 53–187.
  • Fuson KC. Pre-K to grade 2 goals and standards: Achieving 21st century mastery for all. In: Clements DH, Sarama J, DiBiase A-M, editors. Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 105–148.
  • Fuson KC, Kwon Y. Korean childen's understanding of multidigit addition and subtraction. Child Development. 1992; 63 :491–506. [ PubMed : 1611949 ]
  • Gámez PB, Lesaux NK. The relation between exposure to sophisticated and complex language and early-adolescent English-only and language minority learners' vocabulary. Child Development. 2012; 83 (4):1316–1331. [ PubMed : 22591162 ]
  • Gámez PB, Levine SC. Oral language skills of spanish-speaking English language learners: The impact of high-quality native language exposure. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2013; 34 (4):673–696.
  • GAO (U.S. Government Accountability Office). K-12 education school-based physical education and sports programs: Report to congressional requesters. Washington, DC: GAO; 2012.
  • Geary DC. A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1990; 49 :363–383. [ PubMed : 2348157 ]
  • Geary DC. Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychological Bulletin. 1993; 114 :345–362. [ PubMed : 8416036 ]
  • Geary DC. Children's mathematical development: Research and practical applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1994.
  • Geary DC. Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2004; 37 :4–15. [ PubMed : 15493463 ]
  • Geary DC. Cognitive addition: A short longitudinal study of strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in normal and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology. 2011; 47 :1539–1552.
  • Geary DC, Bow-Thomas CC, Yao Y. Counting knowledge and skill in cognitive addition: A comparison of normal and mathematically disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1992; 54 :372–391. [ PubMed : 1453139 ]
  • Geary DC, Hoard MK, Byrd-Craven J, Nugent L, Numtee C. Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development. 2007; 78 :1343–1359. [ PMC free article : PMC4439199 ] [ PubMed : 17650142 ]
  • Geary DC, Hoard MK, Nugent L. Independent contributions of the central executive, intelligence, and in-class attentive behavior to developmental change in the strategies used to solve addition problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012; 113 (1):49–65. [ PMC free article : PMC3392437 ] [ PubMed : 22698947 ]
  • Gelman SA. The essential child origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.
  • Gelman SA, Markman EM. Young children's inductions from natural kinds: The role of categories and appearances. Child Development. 1987; 58 (6):1532–1541. [ PubMed : 3691200 ]
  • Gergely G, Bekkering H, Kiraly I. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature. 2002; 415 (6873):755. [ PubMed : 11845198 ]
  • Gerson SA, Woodward AL. Learning from their own actions: The unique effect of producing actions on infants' action understanding. Child Development. 2014; 85 (1):264–277. [ PMC free article : PMC3740060 ] [ PubMed : 23647241 ]
  • Gilliam WS. Prekindergarteners left behind: Expulsion rates in state prekindergarten systems. New York: Foundation for Child Development; 2005.
  • Girolametto L, Weitzman E. Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions with toddlers and preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2002; 33 (4):268–281. [ PubMed : 27764500 ]
  • Glaser R. The maturing of the relationship between the science of learning of learning and cognition and educational practice. Learning and Instruction. 1991; 1 :129–144.
  • Glasersfeld EV. Sensory experience, abstraction, and teaching. In: Steffe LP, Gale J, editors. Constructivism in education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995. pp. 369–383.
  • Gopnik A, Wellman HM. Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin. 2012; 138 (6):1085–1108. [ PMC free article : PMC3422420 ] [ PubMed : 22582739 ]
  • Gopnik A, Sobel DM, Schulz LE, Glymour C. Causal learning mechanisms in very young children: Two-, three-, and four-year-olds infer causal relations from patterns of variation and covariation. Developmental Psychology. 2001; 37 (5):620–629. [ PubMed : 11552758 ]
  • Gough PB, Tunmer WE. Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education (RASE). 1986; 7 (1):6–10.
  • Gough PB, Hoover WA, Peterson CL. Some observations on a simple view of reading. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J, editors. Reading comprehension difficulties. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. pp. 1–13.
  • Graham SA, Kilbreath CS, Welder AN. Thirteen-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Child Development. 2004; 75 (2):409–427. [ PubMed : 15056196 ]
  • Graziano PA, Reavis RD, Keane SP, Calkins SD. The role of emotion regulation and children's early academic success. Journal of School Psychology. 2007; 45 (1):3–19. [ PMC free article : PMC3004175 ] [ PubMed : 21179384 ]
  • Greenwood C, Buzhardt J, Walker D, Howard W, Anderson R. Program-level influences on the measurement of early communication for infants and toddlers in early Head Start. Journal of Early Intervention. 2011; 33 (2):110–134.
  • Gripshover SJ, Markman EM. Teaching young children a theory of nutrition: Conceptual change and the potential for increased vegetable consumption. Psychological Science. 2013; 24 (8):1541–1553. [ PubMed : 23804961 ]
  • Grissmer D, Grimm KJ, Aiyer SM, Murrah WM, Steele JS. Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology. 2010a; 46 (5):1008–1017. [ PubMed : 20822219 ]
  • Grissmer D, Grimm KJ, Aiyer SM, Murrah WM, Steele JS. New school readiness indicators. Charlottesville: University of Virginia, Center for Advanced Study of Teaching and Learning; 2010b. (Research brief).
  • Gunderson EA, Levine SC. Some types of parent number talk count more than others: Relations between parents' input and children's cardinal-number knowledge. Developmental Science. 2011; 14 (5):1021–1032. [ PMC free article : PMC3177161 ] [ PubMed : 21884318 ]
  • Gunderson EA, Gripshover SJ, Romero C, Dweck CS, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC. Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children's motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Development. 2013; 84 (5):1526–1541. [ PMC free article : PMC3655123 ] [ PubMed : 23397904 ]
  • Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002; 27 (1-2):199–220. [ PubMed : 11750779 ]
  • Gunnar MR, Morison SJ, Chisholm K, Schuder M. Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology. 2001; 13 (3):611–628. [ PubMed : 11523851 ]
  • Gweon H, Schulz L. 16-month-olds rationally infer causes of failed actions. Science. 2011; 332 (6037):1524. [ PubMed : 21700866 ]
  • Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009; 13 (2):65–73. [ PMC free article : PMC3575682 ] [ PubMed : 19135405 ]
  • Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2010; 11 (9):651–659. [ PMC free article : PMC2950073 ] [ PubMed : 20725096 ]
  • Hair E, Halle T, Terry-Humen E, Lavelle B, Calkins J. Children's school readiness in the ECLS-K: Predictions to academic, health, and social outcomes in first grade. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2006; 21 (4):431–454.
  • Hamayan EV, Marler B, Sánchez López C, Damico J. Special education considerations for English language learners: Delivering a continuum of services. 2nd. Philadelphia, PA: Caslon Publishing; 2013.
  • Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature. 2007; 450 (7169):557–559. [ PubMed : 18033298 ]
  • Hamre BK. Teachers' daily interactions with children: An essential ingredient in effective early childhood programs. Child Development Perspectives. 2014; 8 (4):223–230.
  • Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory of children's school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development. 2001; 72 :625–638. [ PubMed : 11333089 ]
  • Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Self-reported depression in nonfamilial caregivers: Prevalence and associations with caregiver behavior in child-care settings. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2004; 19 (2):297–318.
  • Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Can instructional and emotional support in the first-grade classroom make a difference for children at risk of school failure? Child Development. 2005; 76 (5):949–967. [ PubMed : 16149994 ]
  • Hannula MM. Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in the development of early mathematical skills. Turku, Finland: University of Turku; 2005.
  • Hannula MM, Räsänen P, Lehtinen E. Development of counting skills: Role of spontaneous focusing on numerosity and subitizing-based enumeration. Mathematical Thinking and Learning. 2007; 9 :51–57.
  • Harris KR, Friedlander BD, Saddler B, Frizzelle R, Graham S. Self-monitoring of attention versus self-monitoring of academic performance: Effects among students with ADHD in the general education classroom. Journal of Special Education. 2005; 39 (3):145–156.
  • Harris PL. Trusting what you're told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press; 2012.
  • Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 1995.
  • Hawthorne K, Gerken L. From pauses to clauses: Prosody facilitates learning of syntactic constituency. Cognition. 2014; 133 (2):420–428. [ PMC free article : PMC4163511 ] [ PubMed : 25151251 ]
  • Hecht SA, Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Raschotte CA. The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computation skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2001; 79 :192–227. [ PubMed : 11343408 ]
  • Heckman JJ. The economics, technology, and neuroscience of human capability formation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007; 104 (33):13250–13255. [ PMC free article : PMC1948899 ] [ PubMed : 17686985 ]
  • Heckman JJ, Pinto R, Savelyev P. Understanding the mechanisms through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult outcomes. American Economic Review. 2013; 103 (6):2052–2086. [ PMC free article : PMC3951747 ] [ PubMed : 24634518 ]
  • Henry GT, Rickman DK. Do peers influence children's skill development in preschool? Economics of Education Review. 2007; 26 (1):100–112.
  • Hertzman C, Boyce WT. How experience gets under the skin to create gradients in developmental health. Annual Review of Public Health. 2010; 31 :329–347. [ PubMed : 20070189 ]
  • Heyman GD, Dweck CS. Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: Their relation and their role in adaptive motivation. Motivation and Emotion. 1992; 16 (3):231–247.
  • Heyman GD, Gelman SA. The use of trait labels in making psychological inferences. Child Development. 1999; 70 (3):604–619. [ PubMed : 10368912 ]
  • Hibel LC, Granger DA, Blair C, Cox MJ. Maternal sensitivity buffers the adrenocortical implications of intimate partner violence exposure during early childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2011; 23 (2):689–701. [ PubMed : 23786704 ]
  • Hill EL. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004; 8 (1):26–32. [ PubMed : 14697400 ]
  • Hindman AH, Connor CM, Jewkes AM, Morrison FJ. Untangling the effects of shared book reading: Multiple factors and their associations with preschool literacy outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2008; 23 (3):330–350.
  • Hinnant JB, O'Brien M, Ghazarian SR. The longitudinal relations of teacher expectations to achievement in the early school years. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2009; 101 (3):662–670. [ PMC free article : PMC2860190 ] [ PubMed : 20428465 ]
  • Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review. 2006; 26 (1):55–88.
  • Hoff E, Naigles L. How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development. 2002; 73 (2):418–433. [ PubMed : 11949900 ]
  • Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012; 16 (3):174–180. [ PubMed : 22336729 ]
  • Hongwanishkul D, Happaney KR, Lee WSC, Zelazo PD. Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005; 28 (2):617–644. [ PubMed : 16144430 ]
  • Hoover WA, Gough PB. The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 1990; 2 (2):127–160.
  • Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychological Bulletin. 2014; 140 (1):256–282. [ PMC free article : PMC3844011 ] [ PubMed : 23607429 ]
  • Howes C, Hamilton CE. Children's relationships with child care teachers: Stability and concordance with parental attachments. Child Development. 1992; 63 (4):867–878. [ PubMed : 1505245 ]
  • Howes C, Hamilton CE, Phillipsen LC. Stability and continuity of child-caregiver and child-peer relationships. Child Development. 1998; 69 (2):418–426. [ PubMed : 9586216 ]
  • Howse RB, Lange G, Farran DC, Boyles CD. Motivation and self-regulation as predictors of achievement in economically disadvantaged young children. Journal of Experimental Education. 2003a; 71 (2):151–174.
  • Howse RB, Calkins SD, Anastopoulos AD, Keane SP, Shelton TL. Regulatory contributors to children's kindergarten achievement. Early Education and Development. 2003b; 14 (1):101–120.
  • Hoyland A, Dye L, Lawton CL. A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutrition Research Reviews. 2009; 22 (2):220–243. [ PubMed : 19930787 ]
  • Hoynes H, Page M, Stevens AH. Can targeted transfers improve birth outcomes?: Evidence from the introduction of the WIC program. Journal of Public Economics. 2011; 95 (7-8):813–827.
  • Huang-Pollock CL, Karalunas SL, Tam H, Moore AN. Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: A meta-analysis of CPT performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012; 121 (2):360–371. [ PMC free article : PMC3664643 ] [ PubMed : 22428793 ]
  • Huffman LC, Mehlinger SL, Kerivan AS. Risk factors for academic and behavioral problems in the beginning of school. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Center; 2000.
  • Hughes C, Ensor R. Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011; 108 :663–676. [ PubMed : 20673580 ]
  • Huntley-Fenner G, Carey S, Solimando A. Objects are individuals but stuff doesn't count: Perceived rigidity and cohesiveness influence infants' representations of small groups of discrete entities. Cognition. 2002; 85 :203–221. [ PubMed : 12169409 ]
  • Huttenlocher J. Language input and language growth. Preventive Medicine. 1998; 27 (2):195–199. [ PubMed : 9578994 ]
  • Huttenlocher J, Jordan NC, Levine SC. A mental model for early arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1994; 123 :284–296. [ PubMed : 7931093 ]
  • Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Cymerman E, Levine S. Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology. 2002; 45 (3):337–374. [ PubMed : 12480478 ]
  • Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology. 2010; 61 (4):343–365. [ PMC free article : PMC2981670 ] [ PubMed : 20832781 ]
  • Hyde DC, Spelke ES. Neural signatures of number processing in human infants: Evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition. Developmental Science. 2011; 14 (2):360–371. [ PMC free article : PMC3050652 ] [ PubMed : 21399717 ]
  • Inagaki K, Hatano G. Vitalistic causality in young children's naive biology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004; 8 (8):356–362. [ PubMed : 15335462 ]
  • IOM (Institute of Medicine). Hunger and obesity: Understanding a food insecurity paradigm: Workshop summary. Troy LM, Miller EA, Olson S, editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011. [ PubMed : 24983070 ]
  • IOM (Institute of Medicine). Educating the student body: Taking physical activity and physical education to school. Kohl HW III, Cook HD, editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2013. [ PubMed : 24851299 ]
  • IOM and NRC (National Research Council). Preventing mental, emotional, and behavioral disorders among young people: Progress and possibilities. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2009. [ PubMed : 20662125 ]
  • Isen A, Rossin-Slater M, Walker WR. Every breath you take—every dollar you'll make: The long-term consequences of the Clean Air Act of 1970. 2014. (National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series No. 19858).
  • Jacoby JW, Lesaux NK. Support for extended discourse in teacher talk with linguistically diverse preschoolers. Early Education and Development. 2014; 25 (8):1162–1179.
  • Jamison KR, Cabell SQ, LoCasale-Crouch J, Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Class–infant: An observational measure for assessing teacher–infant interactions in center-based child care. Early Education & Development. 2014; 25 (4):553–572.
  • Janzen J. Teaching English language learners. Review of Educational Research. 2008; 78 :1010–1038.
  • Jaswal VK. Believing what you're told: Young children's trust in unexpected testimony about the physical world. Cognitive Psychology. 2010; 61 (3):248–272. [ PMC free article : PMC2930108 ] [ PubMed : 20650449 ]
  • Jenks KM, van Lieshout EC, de Moor JM. Cognitive correlates of mathematical achievement in children with cerebral palsy and typically developing children. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012; 82 (1):120–135. [ PubMed : 22429061 ]
  • Jeon L, Buettner CK, Snyder AR. Pathways from teacher depression and childcare quality to child behavioral problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2014; 82 (2):225–235. [ PubMed : 24447005 ]
  • Jiang Y, Ekono M, Skinner C. Basic facts about low-income children: Children under 6 years, 2012. 2014. [January 26, 2015]. http://nccp ​.org/publications/pub_1088 ​.html .
  • Johnson-Pynn JS, Ready C, Beran M. Estimation mediates preschoolers: Numerical reasoning: Evidence against precise calculation abilities; Paper read at Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; April; Atlanta, GA. 2005.
  • Johnston T, Kirby J. The contribution of naming speed to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. 2006; 19 (4):339–361.
  • Jordan NC, Hanich LB, Uberti HZ. Mathematical thinking and learning difficulties. In: Baroody AJ, Dowker A, editors. The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2003. pp. 359–383.
  • Joshi RM, Aaron PG. The component model of reading: Simple view of reading made a little more complex. Reading Psychology. 2000; 21 (2):85–97.
  • Justice LM, Meier J, Walpole S. Learning new words from storybooks: An efficacy study with at-risk kindergartners. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2005; 36 (1):17–32. [ PubMed : 15801505 ]
  • Justice LM, McGinty AS, Zucker T, Cabell SQ, Piasta SB. Bi-directional dynamics underlie the complexity of talk in teacher–child play-based conversations in classrooms serving at-risk pupils. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2013; 28 (3):496–508.
  • Kamins ML, Dweck CS. Person versus process praise and criticism: Implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Developmental Psychology. 1999; 35 (3):835–847. [ PubMed : 10380873 ]
  • Kaplan DW, Brindis CD, Phibbs SL, Melinkovich P, Naylor K, Ahlstrand K. A comparison study of an elementary school-based health center: Effects on health care access and use. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1999; 153 (3):235–243. [ PubMed : 10086399 ]
  • Karmiloff K, Karmiloff-Smith A. Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2001.
  • Kaufman EL, Lord MW, Reese TW, Volkmann J. The discrimination of visual number. American Journal of Psychology. 1949; 62 :498–525. [ PubMed : 15392567 ]
  • Keenan K, Shaw DS, Walsh B, Delliquadri E, Giovannelli J. DSM-III-R disorders in preschool children from low-income families. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997; 36 (5):620–627. [ PubMed : 9136496 ]
  • Kelley SA, Brownell CA, Campbell SB. Mastery motivation and self-evaluative affect in toddlers: Longitudinal relations with maternal behavior. Child Development. 2000; 71 (4):1061–1071. [ PubMed : 11016566 ]
  • Kerr A, Zelazo PD. Development of “hot” executive function: The children's gambling task. Brain and Cognition. 2004; 55 (1):148–157. [ PubMed : 15134849 ]
  • Keyl PM, Hurtado MP, Barber MM, Borton J. School-based health centers. Students' access, knowledge, and use of services. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1996; 150 (2):175–180. [ PubMed : 8556122 ]
  • Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Gouin JP, Hantsoo L. Close relationships, inflammation, and health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010; 35 (1):33–38. [ PMC free article : PMC2891342 ] [ PubMed : 19751761 ]
  • King JC Jr., Stoddard JJ, Gaglani MJ, Moore KA, Magder L, McClure E, Rubin JD, Englund JA, Neuzil K. Effectiveness of school-based influenza vaccination. New England Journal of Medicine. 2006; 355 (24):2523–2532. [ PubMed : 17167135 ]
  • Kipping P, Gard A, Gilman L, Gorman J. Speech and language development chart. 3rd. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.; 2012.
  • Kirkham NZ, Slemmer JA, Johnson SP. Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition. 2002; 83 (2):B35–B42. [ PubMed : 11869728 ]
  • Kishiyama MM, Boyce WT, Jimenez AM, Perry LM, Knight RT. Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009; 21 (6):1106–1115. [ PubMed : 18752394 ]
  • Klibanoff RS, Levine SC, Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Hedges LV. Preschool children's mathematical knowledge: The effect of teacher “math talk.” Developmental Psychology. 2006; 42 :59–69. [ PubMed : 16420118 ]
  • Koenig MA, Doebel S. Children's understanding of unreliability: Evidence for a negativity bias. In: Banaji MR, Gelman SA, editors. Navigating the social world. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 235–240.
  • Koles B, O'Connor E, McCartney K. Teacher–child relationships in prekindergarten: The influences of child and teacher characteristics. Journal of Early Childhood Teacher Education. 2009; 30 (1):3–21.
  • Koponen T, Salmi P, Eklund K, Aro T. Counting and ran: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013; 105 (1):162–175.
  • La Paro KM, Pianta RC. Predicting children's competence in the early school years: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research. 2000; 70 (4):443–484.
  • Ladd GW, Burgess KB. Do relational risks and protective factors moderate the linkages between childhood aggression and early psychological and school adjustment? Child Development. 2001; 72 (5):1579–1601. [ PubMed : 11699688 ]
  • Ladd GW, Kochenderfer BJ, Coleman CC. Friendship quality as a predictor of young children's early school adjustment. Child Development. 1996; 67 (3):1103–1118. [ PubMed : 8706512 ]
  • Ladd GW, Kochenderfer BJ, Coleman CC. Classroom peer acceptance, friendship, and victimization: Distinct relational systems that contribute uniquely to children's school adjustment? Child Development. 1997; 68 (6):1181–1197. [ PubMed : 9418233 ]
  • Ladd GW, Birch S, Buhs E. Children's social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related spheres of influence? Child Development. 1999; 70 :1373–1400. [ PubMed : 10621962 ]
  • Lalonde CE, Chandler MJ. Children's understanding of interpretation. New Ideas in Psychology. 2002; 20 (2-3):163–198.
  • Lan X, Legare CH, Ponitz CC, Li S, Morrison FJ. Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: A cross-cultural analysis of chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011; 108 :677–692. [ PubMed : 21238977 ]
  • Lane K, Barton-Arwood S, Nelson JR, Wehby J. Academic performance of students with emotional and behavioral disorders served in a self-contained setting. Journal of Behavioral Education. 2008; 17 (1):43–62.
  • Laski EV, Siegler RS. Learning from number board games: You learn what you encode. Developmental Psychology. 2014; 50 (3):853–864. [ PubMed : 24099546 ]
  • Lauderdale DS. Birth outcomes for Arabic-named women in California before and after September 11. Demography. 2006; 43 (1):185–201. [ PubMed : 16579214 ]
  • Lavigne JV, Lebailly SA, Hopkins J, Gouze KR, Binns HJ. The prevalence of ADHD, odd, depression, and anxiety in a community sample of 4-year-olds. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2009; 38 (3):315–328. [ PubMed : 19437293 ]
  • Lawrence J, Snow C. Oral discourse and reading. In: Kamil ML, Pearson PD, Moje EB, Afflerbach P, editors. Handbook of reading research. New York: Routledge; 2011.
  • Le Corre M, Van de Walle GA, Brannon EM, Carey S. Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of counting as a representation of the positive integers. Cognitive Psychology. 2006; 52 (2):130–169. [ PubMed : 16364281 ]
  • Leckman JF, March JS. Editorial: Developmental neuroscience comes of age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011; 52 (4):333–338. [ PubMed : 21410471 ]
  • LeDoux J. The emotional brain: Development and psychopathology. New York: Simon & Schuster; 1996.
  • Lee JS. Size matters: Early vocabulary as a predictor of language and literacy competence. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2011; 32 (1):69–92.
  • Lee JS, Ginsburg HP. Preschool teachers' beliefs about appropriate early literacy and mathematics education for low- and middle-socioeconomic status children. Early Education & Development. 2007; 18 (1):111–143.
  • Lee K. Little liars: Development of verbal deception in children. Child Development Perspectives. 2013; 7 (2):91–96. [ PMC free article : PMC3653594 ] [ PubMed : 23687515 ]
  • Leerkes EM, Paradise MJ, O'Brien M, Calkins SD, Lange G. Emotion and cognition processes in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2008; 54 (1):102–124.
  • LeFevre J-A, Berrigan L, Vendetti C, Kamawar D, Bisanz J, Skwarchuk S-L, Smith-Chant BL. The role of executive attention in the acquisition of mathematical skills for children in grades 2 through 4. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013; 114 (2):243–261. [ PubMed : 23168083 ]
  • Lerkkanen M-K, Rasku-Puttonen H, Aunola K, Nurmi J-E. Mathematical performance predicts progress in reading comprehension among 7-year-olds. European Journal of Psychology of Education. 2005; 20 (2):121–137.
  • Levine SC, Suriyakham LW, Rowe ML, Huttenlocher J, Gunderson EA. What counts in the development of young children's number knowledge? Developmental Psychology. 2010; 46 (5):1309–1319. [ PMC free article : PMC2998540 ] [ PubMed : 20822240 ]
  • Lewit EM, Baker LS. School readiness. The Future of Children/Center for the Future of Children, the David and Lucile Packard Foundation. 1995; 5 (2):128–139. [ PubMed : 8528685 ]
  • Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. Developmental Science. 2010; 13 (6):900–906. [ PMC free article : PMC2966022 ] [ PubMed : 20977560 ]
  • Lieberman AF, Chu A, Van Horn P, Harris WW. Trauma in early childhood: Empirical evidence and clinical implications. Development and Psychopathology. 2011; 23 (2):397–410. [ PubMed : 23786685 ]
  • Lien DS, Evans WN. Estimating the impact of large cigarette tax hikes: The case of maternal smoking and infant birth weight. Journal of Human Resources. 2005; 40 (2):373–392.
  • Low M, Farrell A, Biggs BA, Pasricha SR. Effects of daily iron supplementation in primary-school-aged children: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 2013; 185 (17):E791–E802. [ PMC free article : PMC3832580 ] [ PubMed : 24130243 ]
  • Lozoff B. Early iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects consistent with dopaminergic dysfunction. Journal of Nutrition. 2011; 141 (4):740s–746s. [ PMC free article : PMC3056585 ] [ PubMed : 21346104 ]
  • Lozoff B, Castillo M, Clark KM, Smith JB, Sturza J. Iron supplementation in infancy contributes to more adaptive behavior at 10 years of age. Journal of Nutrition. 2014; 144 (6):838–845. [ PMC free article : PMC4018948 ] [ PubMed : 24717366 ]
  • Luby JL. Handbook of preschool mental health: Development, disorders, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2006.
  • Luby JL, Barch DM, Belden A, Gaffrey MS, Tillman R, Babb C, Nishino T, Suzuki H, Botteron KN. Maternal support in early childhood predicts larger hippocampal volumes at school age. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (8):2854–2859. [ PMC free article : PMC3286943 ] [ PubMed : 22308421 ]
  • Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009; 10 (6):434–445. [ PubMed : 19401723 ]
  • Lynam D, Moffitt T, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school failure, or self-control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993; 102 (2):187–196. [ PubMed : 8315131 ]
  • Lyon GR, Krasnegor NA. Attention, memory, and executive function. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 1996.
  • Maclellan E. Number sense: The underpinning understanding for early quantitative literacy. Numeracy. 2012; 5 (2):1–19.
  • Mages WK. Does creative drama promote language development in early childhood?: A review of the methods and measures employed in the empirical literature. Review of Educational Research. 2008; 78 (1):124–152.
  • Mahoney CR, Taylor HA, Kanarek RB, Samuel P. Effect of breakfast composition on cognitive processes in elementary school children. Physiology and Behavior. 2005; 85 (5):635–645. [ PubMed : 16085130 ]
  • Maier MF, Greenfield DB. The differential role of initiative and persistence in early childhood; Paper presented at Institute of Education Science 2007 Research Conference; Washington, DC. 2008.
  • Mandler JM. The foundations of mind origins of conceptual thought. 2004. [January 2, 2015]. http://site ​.ebrary.com/id/10103678 .
  • Mantzicopoulos P. The relationship of family variables to Head Start children's preacademic competence. Early Education and Development. 1997; 8 (4):357–375.
  • Marcovitch S, Zelazo PD. A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Developmental Science. 2009; 12 (1):1–25. [ PMC free article : PMC2842568 ] [ PubMed : 19120405 ]
  • Markman EM. Realizing that you don't understand: A preliminary investigation. Child Development. 1977; 48 :986–999.
  • Markman EM. Comprehension monitoring. In: Dickson WP, editor. Children's oral communication skills. New York: Academic Press; 1981. pp. 61–84.
  • Marsh HW, Craven R, Debus R. Structure, stability, and development of young children's self-concepts: A multicohort-multioccasion study. Child Development. 1998; 69 (4):1030–1053. [ PubMed : 9768485 ]
  • Marsh HW, Ellis LA, Craven RG. How do preschool children feel about themselves? Unraveling measurement and multidimensional self-concept structure. Developmental Psychology. 2002; 38 (3):376–393. [ PubMed : 12005381 ]
  • Masataka N, Ohnishi T, Imabayashi E, Hirakata M, Matsuda H. Neural correlates for numerical processing in the manual mode. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2006; 11 (2):144–152. [ PubMed : 16319374 ]
  • Mashburn AJ, Pianta RC, Hamre BK, Downer JT, Barbarin OA, Bryant D, Burchinal M, Early DM, Howes C. Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and children's development of academic, language, and social skills. Child Development. 2008; 79 (3):732–749. [ PubMed : 18489424 ]
  • Mashburn AJ, Justice LM, Downer JT, Pianta RC. Peer effects on children's language achievement during pre-kindergarten. Child Development. 2009; 80 (3):686–702. [ PubMed : 19489897 ]
  • Masten AS, Herbers JE, Desjardins CD, Cutuli JJ, McCormick CM, Sapienza JK, Long JD, Zelazo PD. Executive function skills and school success in young children experiencing homelessness. Educational Researcher. 2012; 41 (9):375–384.
  • Master A, Markman EM, Dweck CS. Thinking in categories or along a continuum: Consequences for children's social judgments. Child Development. 2012; 83 (4):1145–1163. [ PubMed : 22540868 ]
  • Mazzocco MMM, Hanich LB. Math achievement, numerical processing, and executive functions in girls with Turner syndrome: Do girls with Turner syndrome have math learning disability? Learning and Individual Differences. 2010; 20 :70–81.
  • Mazzocco MMM, Myers GF. Complexities in identifying and defining mathematics learning disability in the primary school-age years. Annuals of Dyslexia. 2003; 53 :218–253. [ PMC free article : PMC2742419 ] [ PubMed : 19750132 ]
  • Mazzocco MMM, Thompson RE. Kindergarten predictors of math learning disability. Learning Disability Quarterly Research and Practice. 2005; 20 :142–155. [ PMC free article : PMC2806680 ] [ PubMed : 20084182 ]
  • McClelland MM, Cameron CE, Connor CM, Farris CL, Jewkes AM, Morrison FJ. Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers' literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43 (4):947–959. [ PubMed : 17605527 ]
  • McCormick MC, Brooks-Gunn J, Buka SL, Goldman J, Yu J, Salganik M, Scott DT, Bennett FC, Kay LL, Bernbaum JC, Bauer CR, Martin C, Woods ER, Martin A, Casey PH. Early intervention in low birth weight premature infants: Results at 18 years of age for the infant health and development program. Pediatrics. 2006; 117 (3):771–780. [ PubMed : 16510657 ]
  • McEwen BS. Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (Suppl. 2):17180–17185. [ PMC free article : PMC3477378 ] [ PubMed : 23045648 ]
  • McGillicuddy-De Lisi AV. The relationship between parents' beliefs about development and family constellation, socioeconomic status, and parents' teaching strategies. In: Laosa LM, Sigel IE, editors. Families as learning environments for children. New York: Plenum Press; 1982. pp. 261–299.
  • McIntyre LL, Blacher J, Baker BL. The transition to school: Adaptation in young children with and without intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2006; 50 (Pt. 5):349–361. [ PubMed : 16629928 ]
  • McLean JF, Hitch GJ. Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1999; 74 :240–260. [ PubMed : 10527556 ]
  • McLeod DB, Adams VM, editors. Affect and mathematical problem solving. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1989.
  • McMillan T. National Geographic Magazine. Aug, 2014. 2014. The new face of hunger; pp. 66–68. 70, 72-74, 77-80, 83-89.
  • Meltzoff AN. Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology. 1995; 31 (5):838–850. [ PMC free article : PMC4137788 ] [ PubMed : 25147406 ]
  • Metsala JL, Walley AC. Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In: Metsala JL, Ehri LC, editors. Word recognition in beginning literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1998. pp. 89–120.
  • Middleton JA, Spanias P. Motivation for achievement in mathematics: Findings, generalizations, and criticisms of the research. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 1999; 30 :65–88.
  • Miller AL, Seifer R, Stroud L, Sheinkopf SJ, Dickstein S. Biobehavioral indices of emotion regulation relate to school attitudes, motivation, and behavior problems in a low-income preschool sample. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006; 1094 :325–329. [ PubMed : 17347370 ]
  • Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 2011; 137 (6):959–997. [ PMC free article : PMC3202072 ] [ PubMed : 21787044 ]
  • Mills CM. Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 (3):404–418. [ PMC free article : PMC3810952 ] [ PubMed : 22889395 ]
  • Mischel W, Shoda Y, Peake PK. The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988; 54 (4):687–696. [ PubMed : 3367285 ]
  • Mitchell-Copeland J, Denham SA, DeMulder EK. Q-sort assessment of child–teacher attachment relationships and social competence in the preschool. Early Education and Development. 1997; 8 (1):27–39.
  • Mix KS, Huttenlocher J, Levine SC. Quantitative development in infancy and early childhood. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002.
  • Mix KS, Sandhofer CM, Baroody AJ. Number words and number concepts: The interplay of verbal and nonverbal processes in early quantitative development. Kail R, editor. New York: Academic Press; Advances in child development and behavior. 2005; 33 :305–345. [ PubMed : 16101121 ]
  • Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000; 41 (1):49–100. [ PubMed : 10945922 ]
  • Mol SE, Bus AG, de Jong MT. Interactive book reading in early education: A tool to stimulate print knowledge as well as oral language. Review of Educational Research. 2009; 79 (2):979–1007.
  • Moll LC, Amanti C, Neff D, Gonzalez N. Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualitative approach to connect homes and classrooms. Theory into Practice. 1992; 31 (2):132–141.
  • Mooji T. Design and implementation of ICT-supported education for highly able pupils; Paper read at European Conference on Educational Research; Helsinki, Finland. 2010.
  • Morgan GA, Harmon RJ, Maslin-Cole CA. Mastery motivation: Definition and measurement. Early Education and Development. 1990; 1 (5):318–339.
  • Morgan PL, Farkas G, Tufis PA, Sperling RA. Are reading and behavior problems risk factors for each other? Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2008; 41 (5):417–436. [ PMC free article : PMC4422059 ] [ PubMed : 18768774 ]
  • Morrow LM, Schickedanz JA. The relationships between sociodramatic play and literacy development. Dickinson DK, Neuman SB, editors. New York and London: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2006; 2
  • Müller U, Lieberman D, Frye D, Zelazo PD. Executive function, school readiness, and school achievement. In: Fiorello C, Thurman K, editors. Cognitive development in K-3 classroom learning: Research applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2008. pp. 41–84.
  • Mullis IVS, Martin MO, Foy P, Arora A. TIMSS 2011 international results in mathematics. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College, Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study and Progress in International Reading Literacy Study International Study Center; 2012.
  • Munn P. Mathematics in early childhood—the early years math curriculum in the UK and children's numerical development. International Journal of Early Childhood. 2006; 38 (1):99–112.
  • Nachmias M, Gunnar M, Mangelsdorf S, Parritz RH, Buss K. Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment security. Child Development. 1996; 67 (2):508–522. [ PubMed : 8625725 ]
  • Nader-Grosbois N, Lefèvre N. Self-regulation and performance in problem-solving using physical materials or computers in children with intellectual disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2011; 32 :1492–1505. [ PubMed : 21367576 ]
  • Nagy W, Berninger VW, Abbott RD. Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006; 98 (1):134–147.
  • Nation K, Snowling MJ. Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition. 1999; 70 (1):1. [ PubMed : 10193058 ]
  • National Early Literacy Panel. Developing early literacy: A report of the National Early Literacy Panel. 2008. [December 15, 2014]. http://purl ​.access.gpo.gov/GPO/LPS108121 .
  • National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Foundations for success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Planning, Evaluation and Policy Development; 2008.
  • Nayfeld I, Fuccillo J, Greenfield DB. Executive functions in early learning: Extending the relationship between executive functions and school readiness to science. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013; 26 :81–88.
  • NCTSN (National Child Traumatic Stress Network). Understanding child traumatic stress. 2005. [March 23, 2015]. http://www ​.nctsnet.org ​/sites/default/files ​/assets/pdfs/understanding ​_child_traumatic ​_stress_brochure_9-29-05.pdf .
  • NCTSN Core Curriculum on Childhood Trauma Task Force. The 12 core concepts: Concepts for understanding traumatic stress responses in children and families. Core curriculum on childhood trauma. Los Angeles, CA, and Durham, NC: UCLA-Duke University National Center for Child Traumatic Stress; 2012.
  • Neece CL, Baker BL, Blacher J, Crnic KA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children with and without intellectual disability: An examination across time. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2011; 55 (7):623–635. [ PubMed : 21492290 ]
  • Nelson JR, Benner GJ, Lane K, Smith BW. Academic achievement of K-12 students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Exceptional Children. 2004; 71 (1):59–73.
  • Nes FTv. Young children's spatial structuring ability and emerging number sense. de Universtiteit Utrecht; Utrecht, The Netherlands: 2009. (PhD diss.).
  • Neuenschwander R, Röthlisberger M, Cimeli P, Roebers CM. How do different aspects of self-regulation predict successful adaptation to school? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012; 113 (3):353–371. [ PubMed : 22920433 ]
  • NICHD (Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development) Early Child Care Research Network. Social functioning in first grade: Associations with earlier home and child care predictors and with current classroom experiences. Child Development. 2003; 74 (6):1639–1662. [ PubMed : 14669887 ]
  • NICHD (Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development) Early Child Care Research Network. A day in third grade: A large-scale study of classroom quality and teacher and student behavior. Elementary School Journal. 2005; 105 (3):305–323.
  • NRC (National Research Council). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1998.
  • NRC (National Research Council). Eager to learn: Educating our preschoolers. Bowman BT, Donovan MS, Burns MS, editors. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2001.
  • NRC (National Research Council). Mathematics in early childhood: Learning paths toward excellence and equity. Cross CT, Woods TA, Schweingruber H, editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2009.
  • NRC and IOM. From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA, editors. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000. [ PubMed : 25077268 ]
  • NRC and IOM. Depression in parents, parenting, and children: Opportunities to improve identification, treatment, and prevention. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2009. [ PubMed : 25009931 ]
  • Nunes T, Bryant P, Barros R. The development of word recognition and its significance for comprehension and fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012; 104 (4):959–973.
  • Nyaradi A, Li J, Hickling S, Foster J, Oddy WH. The role of nutrition in children's neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through childhood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013; 7 :97. [ PMC free article : PMC3607807 ] [ PubMed : 23532379 ]
  • O'Connor E, McCartney K. Examining teacher–child relationships and achievement as part of an ecological model of development. American Educational Research Journal. 2007; 44 (2):340–369.
  • O'Connor RE. Teaching word recognition effective strategies for students with learning difficulties. New York: Guilford Press; 2014.
  • O'Connor RE, Jenkins JR. Improving the generalization of sound-symbol knowledge: Teaching spelling to kindergarten children with disabilities. Journal of Special Education. 1995; 29 (3):255–275.
  • O'Connor RE, Jenkins JR, Slocum TA. Transfer among phonological tasks in kindergarten: Essential instructional content. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1995; 2 :202–217.
  • Olson SL, Hoza B. Preschool developmental antecedents of conduct problems in children beginning school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1993; 22 (1):60.
  • O'Neill DK, Pearce MJ, Pick JL. Preschool children's narratives and performance on the peabody individualized achievement test—revised: Evidence of a relation between early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language. 2004; 24 (2):149–183.
  • Oreopoulos P, Stabile M, Walld R, Roos LL. Short-, medium-, and long-term consequences of poor infant health: An analysis using siblings and twins. Journal of Human Resources. 2008; 43 (1):88–138.
  • Ostad SA. Subtraction strategies in developmental perspective: A comparison of mathematically normal and mathematically disabled children. Olivier A, Newstead K, editors. Stellenbosch, South Africa: University of Stellenbosch; Proceedings of the 22nd Conference for the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. 1998; 3 :311–318.
  • Pagani L, Messier S. Links between motor skills and indicators of school readiness at kindergarten entry in urban disadvantaged children. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology. 2012; 2 (1):95.
  • Palmer A, Baroody AJ. Blake's development of the number words “one,” “two,” and “three.” Cognition and Instruction. 2011; 29 :265–296.
  • Parish-Morris J, Mahajan N, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Collins MF. Once upon a time: Parent–child dialogue and storybook reading in the electronic era. Mind, Brain, and Education. 2013; 7 (3):200–211.
  • Passolunghi MC, Vercelloni B, Schadee H. The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability and numerical competence. Cognitive Development. 2007; 22 (2):165–184.
  • Pearson PD, Hiebert EH. National reports in literacy: Building a scientific base for policy and practice. Educational Researcher. 2010; 39 :286–294.
  • Perfetti CA. Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press; 1985.
  • Perfetti CA, Hart L. The lexical quality hypothesis. Verhoeven LT, Elbro C, Reitsma P, editors. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company; Precursors of functional literacy. 2002; 11 :67–86.
  • Piaget J, Szeminska A. The child's conception of number. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1952.
  • Pianta RC. Enhancing relationships between children and teachers. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999.
  • Pianta RC, Stuhlman MW. Conceptualizing risk in relational terms: Associations among the quality of child–adult relationships prior to school entry and children's developmental outcomes in first grade. Educational and Child Psychology. 2004a; 21 (1):32–45.
  • Pianta RC, Stuhlman MW. Teacher–child relationships and children's success in the first years of school. School Psychology Review. 2004b; 33 (3):444–458.
  • Pianta RC, Steinberg MS, Rollins KB. The first two years of school: Teacher–child relationships and deflections in children's classroom adjustment. Development and Psychopathology. 1995; 7 (02):295–312.
  • Piazza M, Izard V, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron. 2004; 44 :547–555. [ PubMed : 15504333 ]
  • Pinhas M, Donohue SE, Woldorff MG, Brannon EM. Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of the approximate number system in preschoolers' processing of spoken number words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014; 26 (9):1891–1904. [ PMC free article : PMC4228473 ] [ PubMed : 24702455 ]
  • Ponitz CC, McClelland MM, Matthews JS, Morrison FJ. A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology. 2009; 45 (3):605–619. [ PubMed : 19413419 ]
  • Praet M, Titeca D, Ceulemans A, Desoete A. Language in the prediction of arithmetics in kindergarten and grade 1. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013; 27 :90–96.
  • Purpura DJ, Hume LE, Sims DM, Lonigan CJ. Early literacy and early numeracy: The value of including early literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011; 110 :647–658. [ PubMed : 21831396 ]
  • Raches D, Mazzocco MMM. Emergence and nature of mathematical difficulties in young children with Barth syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2012; 33 (4):328–335. [ PubMed : 22566029 ]
  • Rachlin H. The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2000.
  • Raikes H, Pan BA, Luze G, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Brooks-Gunn J, Constantine J, Tarullo LB, Raikes HA, Rodriguez ET. Mother-child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development. 2006; 77 (4):924–953. [ PubMed : 16942498 ]
  • Ramirez G, Gunderson EA, Levine SC, Beilock SL. Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2013; 14 (2):187–202.
  • Rampersaud GC, Pereira MA, Girard BL, Adams J, Metzl JD. Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic performance in children and adolescents. (quiz 761-742). Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2005; 105 (5):743–760. [ PubMed : 15883552 ]
  • Raver CC. Targeting self-regulation through intervention: Lessons from RCTS; Paper presented at Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness (SREE); September 27; Washington, DC. 2013.
  • Raver CC, Knitzer J. Ready to enter: What research tells policymakers about strategies to promote social and emotional school readiness among three- and four-year-old children. New York: Columbia University, National Center for Children in Poverty, Mailman School of Public Health; 2002.
  • Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Metzger MW, Solomon B. Targeting children's behavior problems in preschool classrooms: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009; 77 (2):302–316. [ PubMed : 19309189 ]
  • Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Bub K, Pressler E. CSRP's impact on low-income preschoolers' preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Development. 2011; 82 (1):362–378. [ PMC free article : PMC3682645 ] [ PubMed : 21291447 ]
  • Reigosa-Crespo V, González-Alemañy E, León T, Torres R, Mosquera R, Valdés-Sosa M. Numerical capacities as domain-specific predictors beyond early mathematics learning: A longitudinal study. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8 (11):e79711. [ PMC free article : PMC3821842 ] [ PubMed : 24255710 ]
  • Rimm-Kaufman SE, Pianta RC, Cox MJ. Teachers' judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2000; 15 (2):147–166.
  • Roberts TA. Home storybook reading in primary or second language with preschool children: Evidence of equal effectiveness for second-language vocabulary acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly. 2008; 43 (2):103–130.
  • Roebers CM, Cimeli P, Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R. Executive functioning, metacognition, and self-perceived competence in elementary school children: An explorative study on their interrelations and their role for school achievement. Metacognition Learning. 2012; 7 (3):151–173.
  • Rogoff B. Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. [ PubMed : 25813118 ]
  • Rogoff B, Angelillo C. Investigating the coordinated functioning of multifaceted cultural practices in human development. Human Development. 2002; 45 (4):211–225.
  • Romine CB, Reynolds CR. A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology. 2005; 12 (4):190–201. [ PubMed : 16422660 ]
  • Romine CB, Lee D, Wolfe ME, Homack S, George C, Riccio CA. Wisconsin card sorting test with children: A meta-analytic study of sensitivity and specificity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2004; 19 (8):1027–1041. [ PubMed : 15533695 ]
  • Rones M, Hoagwood K. School-based mental health services: A research review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2000; 3 (4):223–241. [ PubMed : 11225738 ]
  • Roseberry S, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development. 2014; 85 (3):956–970. [ PMC free article : PMC3962808 ] [ PubMed : 24112079 ]
  • Rossin-Slater M. WIC in your neighborhood: New evidence on the impacts of geographic access to clinics. Journal of Public Economics. 2013; 102 :51–69. [ PMC free article : PMC3772681 ] [ PubMed : 24043906 ]
  • Rothbart MK, Rueda MR. The development of effortful control. In: Mayr U, Awh E, Keele S, editors. Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2005. pp. 167–188.
  • Rouse C, Brooks-Gunn J, McLanahan S. Introducing the issue. The Future of Children. 2005; 15 :5–14.
  • Rowe ML. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development. 2012; 83 (5):1762–1774. [ PMC free article : PMC3440540 ] [ PubMed : 22716950 ]
  • Royer H. Separated at girth: US twin estimates of the effects of birth weight. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2009; 1 (1):49–85.
  • Rutter M. Achievements and challenges in the biology of environmental effects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (Suppl. 2):17149–17153. [ PMC free article : PMC3477381 ] [ PubMed : 23045650 ]
  • Saarni CD, Mumme D, Campos JJ. Emotional development: Action, communication, and understanding. In: Damon W, editor. Handbook of child psychology. 5th. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 237–309.
  • Sadler PM, Tai RH. The two high-school pillars supporting college science. Science. 2007; 317 :457–458. [ PubMed : 17656706 ]
  • Saffran JR. Statistical language learning: Mechanisms and constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2003; 12 (4):110–114.
  • Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science. 1996; 274 (5294):1926–1928. [ PubMed : 8943209 ]
  • Saffran JR, Johnson EK, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition. 1999; 70 (1):27–52. [ PubMed : 10193055 ]
  • Samarapungavan A, Patrick H, Mantzicopoulos P. What kindergarten students learn in inquiry-based science classrooms. Cognition and Instruction. 2011; 29 (4):416–470.
  • Sanders NJ. What doesn't kill you makes you weaker: Prenatal pollution exposure and educational outcomes. Journal of Human Resources. 2012; 47 (3):826–850.
  • Sandhofer CM, Smith LB. Learning color words involves learning a system of mappings. Developmental Psychology. 1999; 35 :668–679. [ PubMed : 10380858 ]
  • Sandler AD, Watson TE, Footo M, Levine MD, Coleman WL, Hooper SR. Neurodevelopmental study of writing disorders in middle childhood. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1992; 13 (1):17–23. [ PubMed : 1556195 ]
  • Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, Glynn LM. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. 2012; 95 (1):8–21. [ PMC free article : PMC7068789 ] [ PubMed : 21494029 ]
  • Sarama J, Clements DH. Early childhood mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for young children. New York: Routledge; 2009.
  • Sarama J, Lange A, Clements DH, Wolfe CB. The impacts of an early mathematics curriculum on emerging literacy and language. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2012; 27 :489–502.
  • Saxe GB, Guberman SR, Gearhart M. Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1987; 52 (2, Serial #216)
  • Saxe R. The new puzzle of theory of mind development. In: Banaji MR, Gelman SA, editors. Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013.
  • Scarborough HS. Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In: Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. Handbook of early literacy research. New York: Guilford Press; 2001. pp. 97–110.
  • Schleppegrell MJ. Grammar for writing: Academic language and the ELD Standards. Santa Barbara, CA: University of California Linguistic Minority Research Institute; 2003.
  • Schneidman LA, Arroyo ME, Levine SC, Goldin-Meadow S. What counts as effective input for word learning? Journal of Child Language. 2013; 40 (3):672–686. [ PMC free article : PMC3445663 ] [ PubMed : 22575125 ]
  • Schoemaker K, Bunte T, Espy KA, Dekovic M, Matthys W. Executive functions in preschool children with ADHD and DBD: An 18-month longitudinal study. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2014; 39 (4):302–315. [ PubMed : 24854774 ]
  • Schore J, Schore A. Modern attachment theory: The central role of affect regulation in development and treatment. Clinical Social Work Journal. 2008; 36 (1):9–20.
  • Schulz LE, Bonawitz EB. Serious fun: Preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43 (4):1045–1050. [ PubMed : 17605535 ]
  • Secada WG. Race, ethnicity, social class, language, and achievement in mathematics. In: Grouws DA, editor. Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning. Toronto and New York: Macmillan, Maxwell Macmillan Canada, Maxwell Macmillan International; 1992. pp. 623–660.
  • Semrud-Clikeman M. The role of inattention on academics, fluid reasoning, and visual-spatial functioning in two subtypes of ADHD. Applied Neuropsychology. Child. 2012; 1 (1):18–29. [ PubMed : 23428274 ]
  • Sénéchal M, Ouellette G, Rodney D. The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary in future reading. Dickinson DK, Neuman SB, editors. New York: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2006; 2 :173–184.
  • Seo K-H, Ginsburg HP. What is developmentally appropriate in early childhood mathematics education? In: Clements DH, Sarama J, DiBiase A-M, editors. Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 91–104.
  • Shayer M, Adhami M. Realizing the cognitive potential of children 5-7 with a mathematics focus: Post-test and long-term effects of a 2-year intervention. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2010; 80 (3):363–379. [ PubMed : 20070920 ]
  • Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009; 301 (21):2252–2259. [ PubMed : 19491187 ]
  • Shuai L, Chan RC, Wang Y. Executive function profile of Chinese boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Different subtypes and comorbidity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2011; 26 (2):120–132. [ PubMed : 21177762 ]
  • Siegel LS, Mazabel S. Basic cognitive processes and reading disabilities. In: Swanson HL, Harris KR, Graham S, editors. Handbook of learning disabilities. New York: Guilford Press; 2013. pp. 186–213.
  • Silver RB, Measelle JR, Armstrong JM, Essex MJ. Trajectories of classroom externalizing behavior: Contributions of child characteristics, family characteristics, and the teacher–child relationship during the school transition. Journal of School Psychology. 2005; 43 (1):39–60.
  • Simmons FR, Willis C, Adams A-M. Different components of working memory have different relationships with different mathematical skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012; 111 (2):139–155. [ PubMed : 22018889 ]
  • Skinner EA, Belmont MJ. Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993; 85 (4):571–581.
  • Slaughter V, Itakura S, Kutsuki A, Siegal M. Learning to count begins in infancy: Evidence from 18 month olds' visual preferences. Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 2011; 278 (1720):2979–2984. [ PMC free article : PMC3151703 ] [ PubMed : 21325331 ]
  • Smiley PA, Dweck CS. Individual differences in achievement goals among young children. Child Development. 1994; 65 (6):1723–1743. [ PubMed : 7859551 ]
  • Smith MW, Dickinson DK. Describing oral language opportunities and environments in Head Start and other preschool classrooms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 1994; 9 (3-4):345–366.
  • Snow CE, Uccelli P. The challenge of academic language. In: Olson DR, Torrance N, editors. In Cambridge handbook of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 2009. pp. 112–133.
  • Snow CE, Tabors PO, Dickinson DK. Language development in the preschool years. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, editors. Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 2001. pp. 1–25.
  • Spelke ES, Kinzler KD. Core knowledge. Developmental Science. 2007; 10 (1):89–96. [ PubMed : 17181705 ]
  • Stanovich KE, Siegel LS. Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1994; 86 :24–53.
  • Starr A, Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Infancy. 2013. Infants show ratio-dependent number discrimination regardless of set size; pp. 1–15. [ PMC free article : PMC3864890 ] [ PubMed : 24353478 ]
  • Steacy LM, Kirby JR, Parrila R, Compton DL. Classification of double deficit groups across time: An analysis of group stability from kindergarten to second grade. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2014; 18 (4):255–273.
  • Steffe LP, Tzur R. Interaction and children's mathematics. Journal of Research in Childhood Education. 1994; 8 (2):99–116.
  • Sternberg R. Beyond IQ. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 1985.
  • Stevenson HW, Newman RS. Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Development. 1986; 57 :646–659. [ PubMed : 3720396 ]
  • Stipek D, Recchia S, McClintic S. Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1992; 57 (1):1–98. [ PubMed : 1560797 ]
  • Strouse GA, O'Doherty K, Troseth GL. Effective coviewing: Preschoolers' learning from video after a dialogic questioning intervention. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 (12):2368–2382. [ PubMed : 23544859 ]
  • Szűcs D, Devine A, Soltesz F, Nobes A, Gabriel F. Cognitive components of a mathematical processing network in 9-year-old children. Developmental Science. 2014; 17 (4):506–524. [ PMC free article : PMC4253132 ] [ PubMed : 25089322 ]
  • Taras H. Nutrition and student performance at school. Journal of School Health. 2005; 75 (6):199–213. [ PubMed : 16014126 ]
  • Thayer ZM, Kuzawa CW. Early origins of health disparities: Material deprivation predicts maternal evening cortisol in pregnancy and offspring cortisol reactivity in the first few weeks of life. American Journal of Human Biology. 2014; 26 (6):723–730. [ PubMed : 24599586 ]
  • Thompson RA. The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, self, conscience. Damon W, Lerner RM, editors. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; Handbook of child psychology. (6th) 2006; 3 :24–98.
  • Thompson RA. Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new answers. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 348–365.
  • Thompson RA. Whither the preconventional child? Toward a life-span moral development theory. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6 (4):423–429.
  • Thompson RA. Attachment theory and research: Precis and prospect. Zelazo P, editor. New York: Oxford University Press; Oxford handbook of developmental psychology. 2013; 2 :191–216.
  • Thompson RA. Stress and child development. The Future of Children. 2014; 24 (1):41–59. [ PubMed : 25518702 ]
  • Thompson RA. The development of virtue: A perspective from developmental psychology. In: Snow NE, editor. Cultivating virtue: Perspectives from philosophy, theology, and psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2015.
  • Thompson RA. Early attachment and later development: Reframing the questions. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment. 3rd. New York: Guilford Press; in press.
  • Thompson RA, Goodvin R. Social support and developmental psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, editor. Developmental psychopathology. 3rd. New York: John Wiley & Sons; in press.
  • Thompson RA, Laible DJ, Ontai LL. Early understandings of emotion, morality, and self: Developing a working model. Advances in Child Development and Behavior. 2003; 31 :137–171. [ PubMed : 14528661 ]
  • Thomson S, Rowe K, Underwood C, Peck R. Numeracy in the early years: Project good start. Camberwell, Victoria, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research; 2005.
  • Toll SWM, Van der Ven S, Kroesbergen E, Van Luit JEH. Executive functions as predictors of math learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2010; 20 (10):1–12.
  • Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. (discussion 691-735). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005; 28 (5):675–691. [ PubMed : 16262930 ]
  • Tomlinson HB. An overview of development in the primary grades. In: Copple C, Bredekamp S, Koralek DG, Charner K, editors. Developmentally appropriate practice. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children; 2014. pp. 9–38.
  • Trentacosta CJ, Izard CE. Kindergarten children's emotion competence as a predictor of their academic competence in first grade. Emotion. 2007; 7 (1):77–88. [ PubMed : 17352565 ]
  • Trick LM, Pylyshyn ZW. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently?: A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review. 1994; 101 :80–102. [ PubMed : 8121961 ]
  • Troseth GL, Saylor MM, Archer AH. Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development. 2006; 77 (3):786–799. [ PubMed : 16686801 ]
  • Tseng V, Seidman E. A systems framework for understanding social settings. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2007; 39 (3-4):217–228. [ PubMed : 17436080 ]
  • Tunmer W, Hoover W. Components of variance models of language-related factors in reading disability: A conceptual overview. In: Joshi RJ, Leong CK, editors. Reading disabilities: Diagnosis and component processes. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer; 1993. pp. 135–173.
  • Tymms P, Merrell C. ADHD and academic attainment: Is there an advantage in impulsivity? Learning and Individual Differences. 2011; 21 (6):753–758.
  • Uccelli P, Hemphill L, Pan BA, Snow C. Conversing with toddlers about the nonpresent: Precursors to narrative development in two genres. In: Balter L, Tamis-LeMonda CS, editors. Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues. 2nd. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2006. pp. 215–237.
  • Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2009; 10 (6):397–409. [ PMC free article : PMC4240627 ] [ PubMed : 19469025 ]
  • U.S. Department of Education Office for Civil Rights. Civil rights data collection: Data snapshot (school discipline). 2014. [February 9, 2015]. (Issue brief no. 1). http://www2 ​.ed.gov/about ​/offices/list/ocr ​/docs/crdc-discipline-snapshot.pdf .
  • Valiente C, Eisenberg N, Haugen R, Spinrad TL, Hofer C, Liew J, Kupfer AS. Children's effortful control and academic achievement: Mediation through social functioning. Early Education & Development. 2011; 22 (3):411–433. [ PMC free article : PMC3346258 ] [ PubMed : 22573931 ]
  • Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen M. Assessment and realistic mathematics education. Utrecht, The Netherlands: Utrecht University, Freudenthal Institute; 1996.
  • Van der Ven SHG, Kroesbergen EH, Boom J, Leseman PPM. The development of executive functions and early mathematics: A dynamic relationship. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012; 82 (1):100–119. [ PubMed : 22429060 ]
  • van Ijzendoorn MH, Dijkstra J, Bus AG. Attachment, intelligence, and language: A meta-analysis. Social Development. 1995; 4 (2):115–128.
  • van Kleek A. Fostering preliteracy development via storybook-sharing interactions: The cultural context of mainstream family practices. In: Stone CA, Silliman ER, Ehren B, Apel K, editors. Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 2004. pp. 175–208.
  • Van Luit JEH, Van de Rijt BAM. Effectiveness of the additional early mathematics program for teaching children early mathematics. Instructional Science. 1998; 26 :337–358.
  • Vasilyeva M, Waterfall H. Variability in language development: Relation to socioeconomic status and environmental input. Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. New York: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2011; 3 :36–48.
  • Vasilyeva M, Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H. Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology. 2006; 42 (1):164–174. [ PubMed : 16420126 ]
  • Vellutino FR, Tunmer WE, Jaccard JJ, Chen R. Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2007; 11 (1):3–32.
  • Vest JR, Catlin TK, Chen JJ, Brownson RC. Multistate analysis of factors associated with intimate partner violence. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2002; 22 (3):156–164. [ PubMed : 11897459 ]
  • Vieillevoye S, Nader-Grosbois N. Self-regulation during pretend play in children with intellectual disability and in normally developing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2008; 29 (3):256–272. [ PubMed : 17576048 ]
  • Vitiello VE, Greenfield DB, Munis P, George JL. Cognitive flexibility, approaches to learning, and academic school readiness in head start preschool children. Early Education & Development. 2011; 22 (3):388–410.
  • von Suchodoletz A, Gunzenhauser C. Behavior regulation and early math and vocabulary knowledge in German preschool children. Early Education & Development. 2013; 24 (3):310–331.
  • Vukovic RK. Mathematics difficulty with and without reading difficulty: Findings and implications from a four-year longitudinal study. Exceptional Children. 2012; 78 :280–300.
  • Vukovic RK, Lesaux NK. The language of mathematics: Investigating the ways language counts for children's mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013; 115 (2):227–244. [ PubMed : 23563157 ]
  • Vygotsky LS. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cole M, John-Steiner V, Scribner S, Souberman E, editors. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978.
  • Vygotsky LS. Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1986.
  • Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Laughon P, Simmons K, Rashotte CA. Development of young readers' phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993; 85 :83–103.
  • Wagner SW, Walters J. A longitudinal analysis of early number concepts: From numbers to number. In: Forman GE, editor. Action and thought. New York: Academic Press; 1982. pp. 137–161.
  • Wang J, Barrett KC. Mastery motivation and self-regulation during early childhood. In: Barrett KC, Fox NA, Morgan GA, Fidler DJ, Daunhauer LA, editors. Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development new directions and international perspectives. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis Press; 2013. pp. 337–380.
  • Wanless SB, McClelland MM, Tominey SL, Acock AC. The influence of demographic risk factors on children's behavioral regulation in prekindergarten and kindergarten. Early Education & Development. 2011; 22 (3):461–488.
  • Warneken F, Tomasello M. Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science. 2006; 311 (5765):1301–1303. [ PubMed : 16513986 ]
  • Warneken F, Tomasello M. Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy. 2007; 11 (3):271–294. [ PubMed : 33412734 ]
  • Weiner B. An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1986.
  • Weisleder A, Fernald A. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science. 2013; 24 (11):2143–2152. [ PMC free article : PMC5510534 ] [ PubMed : 24022649 ]
  • Weizman ZO, Snow CE. Lexical input as related to children's vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology. 2001; 37 (2):265–279. [ PubMed : 11269394 ]
  • Wellman HM. Developing a theory of mind. In: Goswami UC, editor. The handbook of childhood cognitive development. 2nd. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell; 2011. pp. 258–284.
  • Wellman HM, Woolley JD. From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition. 1990; 35 (3):245–275. [ PubMed : 2364653 ]
  • Welsh JA, Nix RL, Blair C, Bierman KL, Nelson KE. The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2010; 102 (1):43–53. [ PMC free article : PMC2856933 ] [ PubMed : 20411025 ]
  • West KK, M. BL, A KK. Mother–child attachment and cognitive performance in middle childhood: An examination of mediating mechanisms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2013; 28 (2):259–270.
  • Whitehurst GJ, Falco FL, Lonigan CJ, F. JE, DeBarshe BD, Valdex-Menchaca MC, Caulfield M. Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Developmental Psychology. 1988; 24 (4):552–559.
  • Wigfield A, Eccles JS, Schiefele U, Roeser RW, Davis-Kean P. Development of achievement motivation. Damon W, Lerner RM, editors. New York: Wiley; Handbook of child psychology. (6th) 2006; 3 :933–1002.
  • Willoughby MT, Kupersmidt J, Voegler-Lee M, Bryant D. Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011; 36 (2):161–180. [ PMC free article : PMC5555639 ] [ PubMed : 21347919 ]
  • Wright RJ. A study of the numerical development of 5-year-olds and 6-year-olds. Educational Studies in Mathematics. 1994; 26 :25–44.
  • Wulfert E, Block JA, Santa Ana E, Rodriguez ML, Colsman M. Delay of gratification: Impulsive choices and problem behaviors in early and late adolescence. Journal of Personality. 2002; 70 (4):533–552. [ PubMed : 12095190 ]
  • Wynn K. Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature. 1992a; 358 (6389):749–750. [ PubMed : 1508269 ]
  • Wynn K. Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology. 1992b; 24 :220–251.
  • Wynn K, Bloom P, Chiang W-C. Enumeration of collective entities by 5-month-old infants. Cognition. 2002; 83 :B55–B62. [ PubMed : 11934407 ]
  • Xu F, Denison S. Statistical inference and sensitivity to sampling in 11-month-old infants. Cognition. 2009; 112 (1):97–104. [ PubMed : 19435629 ]
  • Yoon JM, Johnson MH, Csibra G. Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008; 105 (36):13690–13695. [ PMC free article : PMC2533251 ] [ PubMed : 18757762 ]
  • Young CB, Wu SS, Menon V. The neuro-developmental basis of math anxiety. Psychological Science Online First. 2012; 23 (5) [ PMC free article : PMC3462591 ] [ PubMed : 22434239 ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yow WQ, Markman EM. Bilingual children's use of paralinguistic cues to determine emotion in speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2011a; 14 (4):562–569.
  • Yow WQ, Markman EM. Young bilingual children's heightened sensitivity to referential cues. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2011b; 12 (1):12–31.
  • Yuill N. A funny thing happened on the way to the classroom: Jokes, riddles, and metalinguistic awareness in understanding and improving poor comprehension in children. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J, editors. Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. pp. 193–220.
  • Zeanah CH. Handbook of infant mental health. New York and London: Guilford Press; 2009.
  • Zelazo PD, Carlson SM. Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6 (4):354–360.
  • Zelazo PD, Lyons KE. The potential benefits of mindfulness training in early childhood: A developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6 (2):154–160.
  • Zelazo PD, Jacques S, Burack JA, Frye D. The relation between theory of mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development. 2002; 11 :171–195.
  • Zelazo PD, Müller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S, Argitis G, Boseovski J, Chiang JK, Hongwanishkul D, Schuster BV, Sutherland A, Carlson SM. The development of executive function in early childhood. (Serial No. 274). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2003; 68 (3) [ PubMed : 14723273 ]
  • Zentall SR, Morris BJ. “Good job, you're so smart”: The effects of inconsistency of praise type on young children's motivation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2010; 107 (2):155–163. [ PubMed : 20570281 ]
  • Zero to Three. Diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press; 2005.
  • Ziegler JC, Goswami U. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychonomic Bulletin. 2005; 131 :3–29. [ PubMed : 15631549 ]
  • Zimmerman BJ. Achieving academic excellence: A self-regulatory perspective. In: Ferrar M, editor. The pursuit of excellence through education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 85–110.
  • Zimmerman E, Woolf SH. Understanding the relationship between education and health. 2014. [March 23, 2015]. (Discussion paper). http://www ​.iom.edu/understandingtherelationship .
  • Zins JE, Bloodworth MR, Weissberg RP, Walberg HJ. The scientific base linking social and emotional learning to school success. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation. 2007; 17 (2-3):191–210.
  • Zipke M, Ehri LC, Cairns HS. Using semantic ambiguity instruction to improve third graders' metalinguistic awareness and reading comprehension: An experimental study. Reading Research Quarterly. 2009; 44 (3):300–321.
  • Zucker TA, Cabell SQ, Justice LM, Pentimonti JM, Kaderavek JN. The role of frequent, interactive prekindergarten shared reading in the longitudinal development of language and literacy skills. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 (8):1425–1439. [ PubMed : 23066674 ]

An ongoing study and forthcoming report of the Institute of Medicine and the National Research Council focuses on research, practice, and policy for young dual language learners. More information about this study can be found at www ​.iom.edu/English-DualLanguageLearners .

The 2012 federal poverty threshold was $23,364 for a family of four with two children, $18,480 for a family of three with one child, $15,825 for a family of two with one child.

  • Cite this Page Committee on the Science of Children Birth to Age 8: Deepening and Broadening the Foundation for Success; Board on Children, Youth, and Families; Institute of Medicine; National Research Council; Allen LR, Kelly BB, editors. Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8: A Unifying Foundation. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2015 Jul 23. 4, Child Development and Early Learning.
  • PDF version of this title (11M)

In this Page

Related information.

  • PMC PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed Links to PubMed

Recent Activity

  • Child Development and Early Learning - Transforming the Workforce for Children B... Child Development and Early Learning - Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

Child Development Submission Guidelines

Child Development  invites for consideration manuscripts that are neither identical to nor substantially similar to work published or under review elsewhere. Editors retain the right to reject manuscripts that do not meet established ethical standards for research or dissemination.

Types of Manuscripts

English language editing services, ethics and integrity, format and style, page limits, manuscript structure.

  • Footnotes/Endnotes
  • External Manuscript Resources, Preregistrations, and Data

APA Style Reminders

Note: online supplementary materials, sociocultural policy, prior publication/preprints, methodological recommendations, characterization of studies on a continuum from purely confirmatory to purely exploratory, post-publication data, code, and materials availability and statement of preregistration.

  • Special Sections, Registered Reports, and Commentaries
  • Submission Site
  • Cover Letter

Corresponding Author Responsibilities

Manuscript review, wiley developmental science publishing network, conflicts of interest and the review process, revised manuscript requirements, decision appeals, publication process, note to nih grantees.

  • Media Embargo Information

iThenticate Notification

Child Development considers manuscripts in the formats described below. Inquiries concerning alternative formats should be addressed to the Editor-in-Chief prior to submission. Most submissions (see below) are expected to be no more than 40 manuscript pages, including tables, references, and figures (but excluding appendices). With the exception noted below, if the submission is more than 40 pages, it will be returned to the author for shortening prior to editorial review. Note that we encourage extensive use of electronic supplements that do not count toward the page limit.

Empirical Articles comprise the major portion of the journal. To be accepted, empirical articles must be judged as being high in scientific quality, contributing to the empirical base of child development, and having important theoretical, practical, and/or interdisciplinary implications. Reports of multiple studies, methods, or settings are encouraged, but single-study reports are also considered. Empirical Articles will thus vary considerably in length, but should be no longer than 40 manuscript pages; text and graphics should be as concise as material permits. All modes of empirical research are welcome.

Brief Reports are reserved for short, cutting-edge empirical papers that are no longer than 4,000 words in length (including body text, tables, appendices, etc. but excluding references and electronic supplements), which advance research and knowledge in an area through noteworthy findings and/or new methods.

Reviews focus on past empirical and/or conceptual and theoretical work. They are expected to synthesize, analyze, and/or critically evaluate a topic or issue relevant to child development, should appeal to a broad audience, and may be followed by a small number of solicited commentaries. A large majority of the reviews accepted for publication at Child Development are meta-analyses or invited narrative reviews. Quantitative meta-analyses may be up to 50 pages in length to accommodate sample-specific detail.

Special Sections is a format in which papers on a focal topic, written by different authors, are published simultaneously. In some cases, calls for submissions on particular topics will be disseminated through the SRCD (via e-mail or SRCD publications), and submissions will undergo normal editorial review. In some cases, a submitted manuscript (e.g., an Empirical Article) may be selected as a lead article for this format, with invited commentaries providing additional perspectives. The editors also welcome suggestions from readers for topics for this format. Read additional content requirements for Special Sections .

Commentaries are peer-reviewed papers that respond to previously published Child Development papers. The original paper’s authors have the option to submit a follow-up Commentary in response. Authors must note the paper of reference title, author list and, when available, DOI on the cover page at submission and cover letter. Paper titles are also set as “Commentary: [paper title]”. Read additional content requirements for Commentaries .  Note: Commentaries are approved in advance by the Editor-in-Chief and invited by Associate Editors prior to submission. Do not submit unsolicited Commentary submissions.

Registered Reports are an empirical article in which the conceptualization, methods, and proposed analyses are pre-registered and reviewed prior to data collection and/or analysis being conducted. This format is designed to minimize bias in deductive science, while also allowing flexibility to conduct exploratory (unregistered) analyses and report serendipitous findings. The cornerstone of the Registered Reports format is that the manuscript will be assessed prior to data collection, with the highest quality submissions accepted in advance. Initial submissions will include a description of the key research question and background literature, hypotheses, study design and procedures, analysis pipeline, a statistical power analysis or other sample size rationale, and full description of planned confirmatory data analyses. Pilot data (where applicable) may also be included.

Back to Top

Authors have the option, but are not required, to use the services noted below to translate or proofread their text pre-submission. This option may be particularly helpful to non-native English speakers. Please note that this list is not exhaustive, and SRCD does not endorse – and is not responsible for the quality of – any product or service mentioned.

  • Alba Editing
  • American Journal Experts
  • International Science Editing
  • ScienceDocs
  • Ethical Standards for Research with Children
  • Policy on Scientific Integrity, Transparency, and Openness
  • Author Guidelines on Scientific Integrity and Openness in Child Development

Formatting Requirements

The following points are requested of all papers submitted to Child Development and are required for any paper ultimately accepted for publication. Failure to comply with these requirements may lead to delays in processing, review, or publication. Failure to comply may also lead to the manuscript being returned to you for revision.

Child Development requires that all documents be submitted as Microsoft Word files (.doc or .docx; exceptions may be made by contacting the SRCD Editorial Office).

In addition, all manuscripts must align with APA Style rules including:

  • Double-spacing throughout (abstract, body text, references)
  • Using 12-point, Times New Roman font
  • 1-inch margins
  • When providing racial or ethnic designations, please follow APA's language guidelines. See the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA, 2001, pp. 75–76). Use initial capital letters (i.e., Black and White instead of black and white). Do not use the term Caucasian when describing Whites or people of European descent.

40 pages for Empirical Articles and Reviews, inclusive of everything aside from electronic supplementary materials. The reference list is included in the 40-page total, and may be 8 pages at most. (Quantitative meta-analyses may be up to 50 pages in length).

4,000 words for Empirical Reports, excluding title page, abstract, and references, but inclusive of body text, tables, figures, and appendices.

Empirical Articles and Reports must have the following major sections (other article types may vary):

  • Introduction (but not labeled as such)
  • Tables and Figures ( Child Development strongly encourages authors to embed Tables and Figures in-text upon initial submission)

The Method section must include participant demographic information, such as sex, SES, race or ethnicity, recruitment method, etc.

  • Must be 120 words or fewer
  • Include participants' numerical age
  • Include information about sex/gender and race/ethnicity of participants
  • Include total number of participants ( N s)
  • Should generally report the focal effect size(s), as appropriate
  • Must be written in the third person, not first person
  • Do not exceed 8 pages
  • Are cited both in the body text and on the reference list
  • Are listed in alphabetical order by authors' surname
  • Include the DOI # when available

Color figures publish online and in print free of charge. More technical information on images (accepted file types, image quality, etc.) is available at Wiley-Blackwell Author Services .  Child Development strongly encourages authors to embed Tables and Figures in-text upon initial submission.

Footnotes and Endnotes

Child Development does NOT publish footnotes or endnotes of any kind. All such notes must be incorporated into the body text.

Child Development uses a double-blind reviewing procedure. Please ensure any information that might identify authors is either removed or sufficiently masked.

Information such as the author list, affiliations, acknowledgements, etc. should be removed from the main manuscript file and uploaded as a separate Title Page file during submission.

In-text references to any work by the authors should be referred to in the third person to mask the authors' identities (for example: "We have shown in previous work that children...(Martin, 2011)" should instead be written as "It has been shown in previous work that children...(Martin, 2011)").

External Manuscript Resources, Preregistrations and Data

We encourage authors to share links to research materials, data and preregistrations wherever possible. When authors intend to include these links in their published manuscripts, we ask that an anonymized version be included in the main document submitted for peer review. Repositories such as OSF and AsPredicted offer the facility to create anonymous links.

Child Development follows the Seventh Edition of the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA).

The following are reminders of often forgotten points of APA style. However, ultimately it is the author's responsibility to comply with APA regulations. Failure to follow APA rules may lead to delays in the production process and the publication of your manuscript.

Sexism Avoid sexist language; use plural phrases such as "children and their toys" rather than "a child and his toy." Refrain from referring to children with "it."

Figures Please keep figures as clear and simple as possible. For example, do not use a three-dimensional bar graph unless you are presenting data along three dimensions. Be sure that labels are large enough to be visible when the figure is reduced in size. Remember to provide figure numbers and captions separately, not on the figure itself. Individual figures must be in EPS, PDF, PNG, or TIFF format, with resolution of at least 300 dpi.

"Relationship" vs. "Relation" These are not interchangeable. "Relationship" is used to describe a social bond, such as between a mother and a child, a teacher and a child, etc. "Relation" is used to describe non-animate associations, including those between variables.

Uses of Slash (/) Uses of slash in the abstract and body text must be avoided. Examples include "and/or," his/her," etc. "His/her" can (and should) be written as "his or her." Slashes may be used in references, tables, and figures. Slashes may also be used when citing previously written material, such as including in the paper a test question that was used with participants.

Child Development is able to host supplementary materials to articles published in the journal on its Wiley Online Library website. The current editorial team has been encouraging authors to take advantage of this resource as a way to cut the amount of material included in print articles and to provide additional information to interested readers. As such, we are urging authors to look critically at their manuscripts to find information that could potentially be moved online. Examples of such materials include extra tables, figures, or appendices; test questions or other test materials; videos of experiments taking place; or additional data sets from meta-analyses. For Wiley's guidelines for online supporting materials please see  http://olabout.wiley.com/WileyCDA/Section/id-828014.html .

Additional Content Requirements

  • In quantitative/meta-analytic reviews: tabled information describing key demographic features (sex, ethnicity), the effect size(s), and reliability data for focal measures for each study. Note that demographic data (i.e., sex and ethnicity/race) must be explored as study-level moderators of the focal associations of interest (see No. 1 under “Note for Child Development authors” for additional details).
  • Declaration about the degree to which analyses are relatively exploratory versus confirmatory (often at the end of the Introduction section) ( see Characterization of Studies on a Continuum from Purely Confirmatory to Purely Exploratory ) 
  • Justification of the sample demographic characteristics (racial/ethnic, gender, etc.) and/or justification for why certain socio-demographic data could not be collected (usually in Methods).
  • Generalizability statement, usually within Discussion section. It may be appropriate to describe to what extent sampling might constrain or support generalizability of focal results OR to note how an intentional focus on a particular sociocultural group/groups builds on our knowledge in a given developmental domain in light of the limitations of published research.

As developmental science becomes more global, and the role of context in human development becomes more evident, it is necessary that SRCD publications provide, in addition to age, an indication of the unique characteristics of the sample and the “socioeconomic and cultural place” from which their findings originate. Accordingly, it is now required that manuscripts to be published in SRCD journals specify clearly in the appropriate section(s) (e.g., Method, Discussion) and in an abbreviated form in the Abstract: (1) the dates of data collection (if applicable); (2) the theoretically relevant characteristics of the particular sample studied, for example, but not limited to: race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, language, sexual orientation, gender identity (inclusive of non-binary options), religion, generation, family characteristics; and (3) the place(s) from which that sample was drawn, including country, region, city, neighborhood, school, etc. and all other context variables that are relevant to the focus of the publication, except when it violates expectations of privacy and confidentiality by an institutional review board or the setting itself. Additionally, selection and recruitment procedures should be clearly specified in the Method section.

The Sociocultural Policy is the product of a recognition that current policies and practices were not reflecting the state of the scholarship in terms of addressing diversity and replicability. As such, the Sociocultural Policy reflects current gaps in the science and is a dynamic policy. The Society will conduct ongoing reviews and re-evaluations of the Sociocultural Policy’s effectiveness over time and its efficacy in advancing the Society’s strategic goals. The Sociocultural Policy, procedures, and rationale will be revisited on a biannual basis to reflect changing demographics, an increasingly global society, and relevant contemporary issues.

Note for  Child Development  authors: 

Under the stewardship of prior Editors-in-Chief, Child Development has become—and we intend to remain—a leader in the area of diversity and inclusion, broadly construed, in terms of: (a) the science we publish, (b) our Editorial Board (Associate Editors and Consulting Editors), and (c) those who provide vital service as reviewers.  Furthermore, we strongly encourage the submission of work: (a) focused on diverse populations that (b) covers the full range of substantive issues in child development research, (c) using a wide variety of methods, and (d) submitted by authors who represent a cross-section of the SRCD and child development research more broadly, including international voices.  As such, in addition to following the SRCD-wide Sociocultural policy noted above, under Editor-in-Chief Glenn I. Roisman  Child Development  has instituted the following policies:

  • As noted in these instructions as of July 2019, in the adjudication of manuscripts, sociocultural generalizability or its absence will not be assumed on the basis of the demographic characteristics of a single sample. Sociocultural generalizability of scientific findings (or the lack thereof) is necessarily a product of direct comparisons across demographically diverse samples, and Child Development  encourages manuscripts reporting explicit comparisons of two or more groups to explore generalizability of key developmental phenomena , even if the focal phenomenon is generally regarded as well established in one cultural or other context. (Note that this should not be misconstrued as implying that  Child Development  does not publish studies based on single racial/ethnic groups, as the journal does publish such studies.) In addition, the editorial board of  Child Development  expects that the default position for quantitative (i.e., meta-analytic) reviews will be to include tabled information briefly describing key demographic features of the studies synthesized, along with explicit, even if exploratory examination of such key demographics (i.e., sex and ethnicity/race) as study-level moderators of the focal associations of interest . This table, which should also minimally include effect size and reliability data for focal measures for each sample, may appear in supplementary, electronic materials.  
  • a description of the racial/ethnic, sex/gender, and other demographics of the sample(s) in keeping with SRCD policy above;
  • a brief  justification  of the racial/ethnic, gender, and other demographic characteristics of the sample(s) , including a justification for why certain socio-demographic data could not be collected, when necessary;
  • a brief statement regarding generalizability , usually included within the Discussion section. It may be appropriate to describe in this statement to what extent sampling might constrain or support generalizability of focal results OR to note how an intentional focus on a particular sociocultural group or groups builds on our knowledge in a given developmental domain in light of the limitations of published research.
  • demographic information about who the participants are (basic demographics, including information about age, sex/gender, and race/ethnicity)
  • how many participated (sample size), and, if applicable,
  • a characterization of the focal effect size (generally as a d or r for quantitative studies).

Submissions not conforming to these requirements will be returned to authors for revision prior to consideration by the Editor-in-Chief (EIC). Nonetheless, action editors (including the EIC) will be sensitive in the application of these requirements to the different challenges and constraints that operate internationally in the collection and presentation of demographic data. For example, in some limited instances it may be necessary to describe the population from which the sample was drawn and/or to describe cultural proscriptions related to the collection of such data.

Submitted manuscripts must not have been published elsewhere. In general, preprints (e.g., on a departmental website, on PsyArXiv) will not count as prior publication. However, if the document has a copyright license (including Creative Commons), or any other constraints on editing or publication of the work, this information must be disclosed and addressed in the cover letter.  It is the responsibility of authors to provide sufficient information about publicly available versions of the manuscript to allow editors to make informed decisions about prior publication .

Please note that once a manuscript is accepted for publication, SRCD policy requires that any previous versions be labeled as “drafts” or “working papers.” There must be some designation indicating that those documents are not “the version of record.” If restrictions on prior versions preclude editing to add this designation, SRCD will likely be unable to accept the submission.

In an effort to further Child Development’s tradition of publishing rigorous research, we previously announced methodological author recommendations to support the production of accessible and reproducible, high-quality research without excluding innovative hypothesis-generating inquiry. To satisfy these recommendations, we ask that authors address sample recruitment and selection, data collection and coding, descriptive statistical information, and model misspecification in the body of their manuscripts or as online supplements. View the full descriptions of the recommendations and further information.

As implied in the Methodological Recommendations described immediately above, published reports in Child Development lie on a continuum between more exploratory, “discovery” science and more confirmatory hypothesis testing. Nonetheless, locating a given contribution on that continuum can be difficult for even the most informed reader. Submitting authors to Child Development are therefore expected to clearly describe in their manuscripts to what degree the analyses presented represent a relatively exploratory versus confirmatory effort, and in what ways. For example, pre-registered reports of large, well-defined samples that involve point or point-like predictions would generally be viewed as more confirmatory whereas research in a novel domain with smaller samples and with non-directional hypotheses or perhaps with no initial hypotheses tend to be more exploratory.

In short, the Editorial Board requires that submissions carefully and accurately describe the relatively exploratory versus confirmatory nature of the analyses presented, and, indeed, whether the nominally significant results highlighted in these manuscripts are consistent with sampling error. With that said,  Child Development  encourages and values the publication of principled exploratory research as well as more confirmatory efforts, including high fidelity replication studies focused on high impact findings. 

All manuscripts submitted to Child Development must be in compliance with the SRCD's Author Guidelines on Scientific Integrity and Openness in Child Development . Consistent with this SRCD Policy, Child Development further requires that the following information be provided in the Acknowledgments section of all manuscripts submitted and ultimately published in the journal. Note that this information will not be used as part of the review process in that ultimate acceptance/rejection of a submitted manuscript is not conditional on the answers to the following questions. However, as a separate matter, action editors may request data and/or code as part of the review process (without the expectation that those will be made available post-publication).

First, please indicate in Acknowledgements one of the following regarding data availability :

“The data necessary to reproduce the analyses presented here are not publicly accessible.”

“The data necessary to reproduce the analyses presented here are publicly accessible.”

If data are or will be made publicly available, also provide information as to how to access the data. For example: “Data are available from the first author upon reasonable request.” Or: “Data are available at the following URL: http://www.example.org ”

Second, please indicate in Acknowledgements one of the following regarding analytic code :

“The analytic code necessary to reproduce the analyses presented in this paper is not publicly accessible.”

“The analytic code necessary to reproduce the analyses presented in this paper is publicly accessible.”

If code is or will be made publicly available, also provide information as to how to access the code. For example: “Code is available from the first author.” Or: “Code is available at the following URL: http://www.example.org ”

Third, please indicate in Acknowledgements one of the following regarding materials:

“The materials necessary to attempt to replicate the findings presented here are not publicly accessible.”

“The materials necessary to attempt to replicate the findings presented here are publicly accessible”

If materials are or will be made publicly available, also provide information as to how to access the materials. For example: “Materials are available from the first author.” Or: “Materials are available at the following URL: http://www.example.org ”

Fourth and finally, please indicate in Acknowledgements one of the following regarding preregistration :

“The analyses presented here were not preregistered.”

“The analyses presented here were preregistered.”

If analyses were preregistered, also provide information as to how to access the preregistration. For example: “The preregistration is available at the following URL: http://www.example.org ”

It is acceptable to combine the steps above for brevity. For example: “The data and code necessary to reproduce the analyses presented here are publicly accessible, as are the materials necessary to attempt to replicate the findings. Analyses were also pre-registered. Data, code, materials, and the preregistration for this research are available at the following URL: http://www.example.org .”

Special Sections, Registered Reports, & Commentaries

In addition to the above manuscript requirements, Special Sections, Registered Reports, and Commentaries require additional materials.

Special Sections

  • Article type must be Special Section on submission
  • Note the submission is part of a Special Section
  • State the full title of the Special Section (not to be confused with the actual submission title)
  • State the Special Section Editor(s)
  • Review individual Special Section announcements for additional instructions specific to the section.

Registered Reports

  • Stage 2 Registered Reports must be submitted as such through the submission portal
  • Note the submission is a Registered Report and part of the SRCD Registered Reports Special Section
  • State if the submission is a Stage 1 or Stage 2 Registered Report
  • See full Author Guidelines for Registered Reports .

Commentaries

  • Note, Commentaries are approved in advance by the Editor-in-Chief and invited by Associate Editors prior to submission. Do not submit unsolicited Commentary submissions . Article type must be Commentary on submission.
  • State the submission is a Commentary
  • State the full title, corresponding author and (if possible) manuscript number of the paper it addresses
  • State if submission is the author response to a separate Commentary and, if so, name the original paper title, manuscript number as well as all above details of initial commentary.
  • State if an editor invited the commentary and the editor’s name. Note almost all Commentaries are approved in advance by the Editor-in-Chief and invited by Associate Editors.
  • Note previous correspondence with the original paper authors

Manuscript Submission and Review Process

How to submit.

Manuscripts should be submitted online through the Child Development ScholarOne Portal.

Submit to Child Development

Full instructions and support are available on the site and a user ID and password can be obtained on the first visit. If for some reason you cannot submit online, please contact the SRCD Editorial Office by telephone (202-800-0668) or by e-mail ( [email protected] ).

Cover letter

Please include a cover letter that contains the name(s) and affiliation(s) of the author(s), and the street address, telephone, fax, e-mail address of the corresponding author. Please also provide details about other published or submitted papers having substantial overlap (including data sets) with the new Child Development submission to enable editors to judge whether the new submission is sufficiently distinct from other work. Please also consider suggesting an Associate Editor to process your manuscript in the cover letter; note, it is not always possible to honor such requests. If submitting a Special Section, Registered Report or Commentary, please observe the additional Cover Letter requirements under Additional Content Requirements .

A corresponding author's submission to Child Development implies that all coauthors have agreed to the content and form of the manuscript and that the ethical standards of the SRCD have been followed (see the SRCD website or pp. 283–284 of the 2000 SRCD Directory). Any financial interest or conflict of interest must be explained in the cover letter. The corresponding author is responsible for informing all coauthors of manuscript submission, editorial decisions, reviews, and revisions.

Child Development conducts a double-blind review process. Each manuscript is handled by the Editor-in-Chief or an Associate Editor who consults with one or more Consulting Editors and/or ad hoc reviewers who have relevant expertise. Cover sheets are removed before review to ensure blind review; authors should avoid including any other information about identity or affiliation in submissions. Copies of the submission and associated correspondence are retained in the SRCD archives.

Prior to formal assignment to an Associate Editor, the Editor-in-Chief, in consultation with members of the Editorial Board, will determine whether a submitted manuscript is likely to be competitive for publication in the journal. Desk rejection is a common outcome for manuscripts submitted to the journal, which speeds up feedback to authors and creates fewer burdens for reviewers. Studies based on underrepresented groups and/or drawn from diverse populations will be regarded as strong candidates to advance to further consideration by an Associate Editor. 

Manuscripts that pass through an initial review by the Editor-in-Chief (which will generally take no longer than 1 week) are assigned to an Associate Editor, who will in turn generally invite 2-3 consultants who have pertinent areas of expertise to review the manuscript. Authors are encouraged to recommend possible reviewers during the submission process, but this is neither required nor are the editors required to abide by the recommendations. Associate Editors also reserve the right to desk reject a manuscript they regard as not competitive for publication in the journal.

Once the Associate Editor receives the requested number of reviews they will make an editorial decision in part based on the reviews and reviewer recommendations, along with their own editorial judgement. The Associate Editor’s decision letter, and accompanying reviews, are blinded and processed by the SRCD Editorial Office staff. These materials are then sent to the authors and all reviewers who contributed to the review process.

Consistent with the recently adopted Author Guidelines on Scientific Integrity and Openness for Publishing in Child Development : (a) providing data and/or code used to produce a manuscript published in Child Development to other researchers who wish to reproduce those findings or include them in a meta-analysis is strongly encouraged and (b) information about whether an author intends to share data and/or code post-publication will be collected post-acceptance, prior to publication. Whether or not authors agree to make data available and/or make their analytic methods (e.g., code) available post -publication, however, will not affect the review/acceptance decision. Nonetheless, at Child Development the action editors reserve the right to independently determine whether it is necessary for an author to provide data and/or code pre -publication to reviewers and/or the action editor in order for the Editor-in-Chief or Associate Editor to render a decision on a manuscript.

Child Development strives to deliver decisions within 60 days of submission. However, given the nature of the review process turnaround times may vary. If you have any questions about your submission, please inquire at [email protected] or call (202) 800-0668.

Submissions to Child Development by current Associate Editors and the Editor-in-Chief are handled independently by a Special Associate Editor.

Child Development  participates in the Wiley Developmental Science Publishing Network . This exciting collaboration between a number of high quality journals simplifies and speeds up the publication process, helping authors find a home for their research. At the Editors’ judgement, suitable papers not accepted by one journal may be recommended for referral to another journal(s) in the network. Authors decide whether to accept the referral, with the option to transfer their paper with or without revisions. Once the referral is accepted, submission happens automatically, along with any previous reviewer reports, thereby relieving pressure on the peer review process.  While a transfer does not guarantee acceptance, it is more likely to lead to a successful outcome for authors by helping them to find a route to publication quickly and easily.

Child Development takes conflicts of interest (COIs) very seriously. COIs arise when scholars are asked to review papers of which their opinions could be biased. In order to avoid any COIs in the manuscript review process, we ask that authors NOT recommend as a reviewer any person who:

  • Is a close personal friend of any of the authors
  • Works in the same department or school as any of the authors
  • Has recently collaborated with any of the authors on projects or publications (this includes members of the authors' dissertation committees)
  • Was a recent Ph.D. student advised by any of the authors
  • Has any other relationship with the authors that could bias their opinion of the submitted manuscript

We also ask that anyone invited to review a manuscript inform the SRCD Editorial Office if they believe they have a conflict of interest. If you have any questions about potential COIs, please contact the SRCD Editorial Office ( [email protected] ).

When preparing a revised manuscript, authors will be required to run StatCheck  on their manuscripts (a free, easy to use, web-based version is available at StatCheck.io ). Authors are also provided the Conflict of Interest and Public Summary forms when their paper is returned for revision. Papers cannot move forward from this stage without completed forms.  Completion of forms does not guarantee acceptance.

Authors will also be required to affirm in the updated cover letter to the Associate Editor:

  • that the revised manuscript has no inconsistent p -values as detected by StatCheck
  • the page number where the Confirmatory/Exploratory statement is stated, and
  • the page number where the Sociocultural Statement is stated. Note, authors who cannot comply with the journal sociocultural policy (legal constraints, etc.) must state as much in the revised cover letter.”

The following file types should be provided for revised manuscripts:

  • The main text as a Microsoft Word document with figures and tables removed and notes in-text stating where they should appear on publication ([Table 1 goes here])
  • Individual high-resolution figure files as  EPS, TIFF, or PDFs  clearly labeled
  • Individual table files as  Microsoft Word documents  clearly labelled

Authors may appeal a manuscript decision by emailing both the Action Editor who handled the manuscript and the Editor-in-Chief of the journal, cc’ing the Editorial Office. The email message must clearly state the case for why the decision should be changed. Appeals will only be considered if the authors: (a) identify factual errors made by the reviewers or Editor that had a major impact on the decision, or (b) can provide a substantiated claim of unfair treatment and/or bias in the review process. Appeals for any other reason will be denied without further consideration. Appeals that meet the identified criteria will be considered by the original Action Editor. Concerns may be escalated to the Editor-in-Chief, but only after a decision on an appeal is rendered by the Action Editor.

Accepted Manuscript and Publication Information

Once the above materials have been received the paper will be scheduled to be sent to our publisher Wiley-Blackwell for typesetting and proofing. It will then publish online to W-B’s Early View system, with print publication to follow.

Pursuant to NIH mandate, the SRCD through Wiley-Blackwell will post the accepted version of contributions authored by NIH grant holders to PubMed Central upon acceptance. This accepted version will be made publicly available 12 months after publication. For further information, see  www.wiley.com/go/nihmandate .

SRCD Policy on Media Embargo Dates

Powered by Turnitin’s text comparison tool iThenticate, CrossRef’s Similarity Check enables the SRCD Editorial Office to assess the originality of all submitted manuscripts. iThenticate finds and reports overlaps in text through the use of text-searching algorithms, checking a document against an extensive database of published scholarly writing. Its reports highlight material that matches documents in the iThenticate database and their percentage of similarity. The reports allow editors to judge the appropriateness of any substantial overlap between documents, which can range from authors using identical language across manuscripts to report precise details of methods to more concerning cases of recycling text.

For more information, please see the Committee on Publication Ethics' Text Recycling Guidelines: https://publicationethics.org/text-recycling-guidelines .

General Inquiries

Call today to schedule an appointment or fill out an online request form. If requested before 2 p.m. you will receive a response today.

713-798-1000

Monday - Friday 8 a.m. - 5 p.m.

Request Now

Request non-urgent appointments

Request an appointment, learn about your rights as a patient, read about what to expect from your appointment, and more.

As Houston's premier academic medical practice, Baylor Medicine delivers compassionate, innovative, evidence-based care. Find a Doctor

get-to-know-houston

America's fourth largest city is a great place to live, work and play. Find out why. Get to Know Houston

linkedin

Give Careers Intranet

facebook

  • Baylor College of Medicine

The importance of socializing children

sfsf

Homa Warren

713-798-4710

Share this article

linkedin

Socialization is crucial to children’s development from an early age, shaping their skills and fostering companionship and empathy. Relationships teach young children about perspective, taking turns and meaningful friendships. A Baylor College of Medicine psychologist explains the importance of socialization for children while offering tips for parents of children who struggle in social settings. 

“[Socialization] gives the benefit of having peer support and friendship that can bolster sense of self,” said Dr. Eric Storch , professor and vice chair of psychology in the Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at Baylor. “Friends not only provide emotional support, but also give feedback that, together with other important figures, helps shape the person you become.” 

Social interaction is necessary from day one. For babies and small children, spending time with family is crucial for development. As they start to age, they shift their social interaction to focus on peers and friends, while keeping family close. 

While there is no recipe for the perfect amount of socialization for a child, parents should focus on their children if they’re struggling in certain social situations and understand how they engage with others. This may be driven by anxiety or simply different interests. It might also stem from the pandemic, which impacted younger and older children. While most children were resilient and bounced back, some suffered more than others and are experiencing behavioral health issues. 

Storch suggests parents and caregivers, especially of children with behavioral health issues, take steps to help their child succeed in social situations:

  • Facilitate opportunities for children to be themselves. Every child is different; one might enjoy playing sports, while another might play Dungeons and Dragons. Personalize ways they can engage with others. 
  • Encourage variety and risk taking. Making sure your child gets out of their comfort zone in a safe fashion is helpful. 
  • Join groups . Group settings can facilitate different interactional and socialization skills. 
  • Consult a mental health professional. When you’re worried about a child’s ability to effectively socialize or you sense behavioral health concerns, such as anxiety and depression, reaching out to a doctor or mental health professional is beneficial. 

back-to-top

  • Introduction
  • Conclusions
  • Article Information

PT indicates physical therapy; RAGT, robot-assisted gait training.

Trial Protocol

eFigure 1. The Torque-Assisting Wearable Exoskeletal Robot

eFigure 2. Study Flowchart

eAppendix. Conventional Physical Therapy (PT) for Control Group

Data Sharing Statement

See More About

Sign up for emails based on your interests, select your interests.

Customize your JAMA Network experience by selecting one or more topics from the list below.

  • Academic Medicine
  • Acid Base, Electrolytes, Fluids
  • Allergy and Clinical Immunology
  • American Indian or Alaska Natives
  • Anesthesiology
  • Anticoagulation
  • Art and Images in Psychiatry
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Assisted Reproduction
  • Bleeding and Transfusion
  • Caring for the Critically Ill Patient
  • Challenges in Clinical Electrocardiography
  • Climate and Health
  • Climate Change
  • Clinical Challenge
  • Clinical Decision Support
  • Clinical Implications of Basic Neuroscience
  • Clinical Pharmacy and Pharmacology
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Consensus Statements
  • Coronavirus (COVID-19)
  • Critical Care Medicine
  • Cultural Competency
  • Dental Medicine
  • Dermatology
  • Diabetes and Endocrinology
  • Diagnostic Test Interpretation
  • Drug Development
  • Electronic Health Records
  • Emergency Medicine
  • End of Life, Hospice, Palliative Care
  • Environmental Health
  • Equity, Diversity, and Inclusion
  • Facial Plastic Surgery
  • Gastroenterology and Hepatology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Genomics and Precision Health
  • Global Health
  • Guide to Statistics and Methods
  • Hair Disorders
  • Health Care Delivery Models
  • Health Care Economics, Insurance, Payment
  • Health Care Quality
  • Health Care Reform
  • Health Care Safety
  • Health Care Workforce
  • Health Disparities
  • Health Inequities
  • Health Policy
  • Health Systems Science
  • History of Medicine
  • Hypertension
  • Images in Neurology
  • Implementation Science
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Innovations in Health Care Delivery
  • JAMA Infographic
  • Law and Medicine
  • Leading Change
  • Less is More
  • LGBTQIA Medicine
  • Lifestyle Behaviors
  • Medical Coding
  • Medical Devices and Equipment
  • Medical Education
  • Medical Education and Training
  • Medical Journals and Publishing
  • Mobile Health and Telemedicine
  • Narrative Medicine
  • Neuroscience and Psychiatry
  • Notable Notes
  • Nutrition, Obesity, Exercise
  • Obstetrics and Gynecology
  • Occupational Health
  • Ophthalmology
  • Orthopedics
  • Otolaryngology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Care
  • Pathology and Laboratory Medicine
  • Patient Care
  • Patient Information
  • Performance Improvement
  • Performance Measures
  • Perioperative Care and Consultation
  • Pharmacoeconomics
  • Pharmacoepidemiology
  • Pharmacogenetics
  • Pharmacy and Clinical Pharmacology
  • Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
  • Physical Therapy
  • Physician Leadership
  • Population Health
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Well-being
  • Professionalism
  • Psychiatry and Behavioral Health
  • Public Health
  • Pulmonary Medicine
  • Regulatory Agencies
  • Reproductive Health
  • Research, Methods, Statistics
  • Resuscitation
  • Rheumatology
  • Risk Management
  • Scientific Discovery and the Future of Medicine
  • Shared Decision Making and Communication
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports Medicine
  • Stem Cell Transplantation
  • Substance Use and Addiction Medicine
  • Surgical Innovation
  • Surgical Pearls
  • Teachable Moment
  • Technology and Finance
  • The Art of JAMA
  • The Arts and Medicine
  • The Rational Clinical Examination
  • Tobacco and e-Cigarettes
  • Translational Medicine
  • Trauma and Injury
  • Treatment Adherence
  • Ultrasonography
  • Users' Guide to the Medical Literature
  • Vaccination
  • Venous Thromboembolism
  • Veterans Health
  • Women's Health
  • Workflow and Process
  • Wound Care, Infection, Healing

Get the latest research based on your areas of interest.

Others also liked.

  • Download PDF
  • X Facebook More LinkedIn

Choi JY , Kim SK , Hong J, et al. Overground Gait Training With a Wearable Robot in Children With Cerebral Palsy : A Randomized Clinical Trial . JAMA Netw Open. 2024;7(7):e2422625. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2024.22625

Manage citations:

© 2024

  • Permissions

Overground Gait Training With a Wearable Robot in Children With Cerebral Palsy : A Randomized Clinical Trial

  • 1 Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Chungnam National University Hospital, Daejeon, Korea
  • 2 Department and Rehabilitation Medicine, Yongin Severance Hospital, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Yongin, Korea
  • 3 Department and Research Institute of Rehabilitation Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea
  • 4 Department and Rehabilitation Medicine, Seoul Rehabilitation Hospital, Seoul, Korea
  • 5 George W. Woodruff School of Mechanical Engineering, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta
  • 6 Institute for Robotics and Intelligent Machines, Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta
  • 7 Department of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, Chonnam National University Medical School and Hospital, Gwangju, Korea

Question   Is robot-assisted gait training with wearable exoskeletal robots more effective than conventional physical therapy in children with cerebral palsy (CP)?

Findings   This randomized clinical trial of 90 children with CP found significant improvements in gross motor function, balance control, and gait pattern among children receiving robot-assisted gait training.

Meaning   These findings suggest that an untethered, torque-assisted, wearable exoskeletal robot, based on assist-as-needed control, is effective in children with CP.

Importance   Cerebral palsy (CP) is the most common developmental motor disorder in children. Robot-assisted gait training (RAGT) using a wearable robot can provide intensive overground walking experience.

Objective   To investigate the effectiveness of overground RAGT in children with CP using an untethered, torque-assisted, wearable exoskeletal robot.

Design, Setting, and Participants   This multicenter, single-blind randomized clinical trial was conducted from September 1, 2021, to March 31, 2023, at 5 rehabilitation institutions in Korea. Ninety children with CP in Gross Motor Function Classification System levels II to IV were randomized.

Intervention   The RAGT group underwent 18 sessions of RAGT during 6 weeks, whereas the control group received standard physical therapy for the same number of sessions during the same period.

Main Outcome and Measures   The primary outcome measure was the Gross Motor Function Measure 88 (GMFM-88) score. Secondary outcome measures were the GMFM-66, Pediatric Balance Scale, selective control assessment of the lower extremity, Pediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory–Computer Adaptive Test (PEDI-CAT), 6-minute walking test scores (distance and oxygen consumption), muscle and fat mass via bioelectrical impedance analysis, and gait parameters measured via 3-dimensional analysis. All assessments were performed for all patients at baseline, at the end of the 6-week intervention, and after the 4-week follow-up.

Results   Of the 90 children (mean [SD] age, 9.51 [2.48] years; 49 [54.4%] male and 41 [45.6%] female) in the study, 78 (86.7%) completed the intervention, with 37 participants (mean [SD] age, 9.57 [2.38] years; 19 [51.4%] male) and 41 participants (mean [SD] age, 9.32 [2.37] years; 26 [63.4%] male) randomly assigned to the RAGT and control groups, respectively. Changes in the RAGT group significantly exceeded changes in the control group in GMFM-88 total (mean difference, 2.64; 95% CI, 0.50-4.78), GMFM-E (mean difference, 2.70; 95% CI, 0.08-5.33), GMFM-66 (mean difference, 1.31; 95% CI, 0.01-2.60), and PEDI-CAT responsibility domain scores (mean difference, 2.52; 95% CI, 0.42-4.63), indicating independence in daily living at postintervention assessment. At the 4-week follow-up, the RAGT group showed significantly greater improvements in balance control (mean difference, 1.48; 95% CI, 0.03-2.94) and Gait Deviation Index (mean difference, 6.48; 95% CI, 2.77-10.19) compared with the control group.

Conclusions and Relevance   In this randomized clinical trial, overground RAGT using a wearable robot significantly improved gross motor function and gait pattern. This new torque-assisted wearable exoskeletal robot, based on assist-as-needed control, may complement standard rehabilitation by providing adequate assistance and therapeutic support to children with CP.

Trial Registration   CRIS Identifier: KCT0006273

Cerebral palsy (CP) is the most common neuromotor disorder in children, limiting walking and daily activity. 1 Damage to the developing brain in CP leads to abnormal motor experience due to altered neurologic function, including weakness, spasticity, loss of motor control, and limited coordination. 2 The consequential abnormal sensorimotor experiences and movement restrictions adversely affect musculoskeletal changes and cortical disorganization, resulting in a vicious circle. 2

Children with CP have various gait impairments that significantly affect their daily activities and social integration. 3 Therefore, a key therapeutic goal in children with CP is to enhance walking ability. 4 Treadmill training, partial body weight support gait training, and goal-directed training are known to enhance motor function in children with CP. 5 With recent technological advancements, robot-assisted gait training (RAGT) offers constant patterns of repetitive, high-intensity, and goal-oriented training—the basic principle of effective rehabilitation. However, a recent meta-analysis revealed weak and inconsistent evidence of RAGT effects in children with CP. 6 Most devices included in the meta-analysis were based on treadmill-tethered, trajectory-controlled robots. Further research should explore various types of robotic systems to establish more robust clinical evidence for RAGT.

Regarding assistive control strategy, 2 types of RAGT devices can be distinguished: torque control or gait trajectory controllers. The trajectory-controlled robot operates on a kinematic basis along a predefined gait path and set joint angle, regardless of the residual power, which is typically used for complete motor impairment, whereas the torque-controlled model adjusts the amount of force applied at each joint to aid movement based on the user’s effort and motion, requiring active participation from the patient. 7 Most patients with CP can generate some effort at the major joints of the lower limbs, despite varying degrees of motor impairment. As for traditional training methods with gait robots, there are treadmill-tethered robots and overground robots with a wearable suit. The most developed pediatric RAGT devices are treadmill-tethered trajectory-controlled or end-effector robots. However, to our knowledge, no multicenter large-scale randomized clinical trials exist on the effects of overground wearable exoskeleton RAGT in children with CP.

The recently developed torque-assisted wearable RAGT device allows training on various terrains, including overground terrain, ramps, and stairs. This robotic suit can assist joint motion based on assist-as-needed control, potentially promoting active participation. Active engagement, generalization of tasks in the real world, and kinematic variability during gait are essential for effective motor learning. 8 - 10 In contrast, conventional RAGT devices with constant guidance and full support often lead to participants being passive, reducing patient effort and weakening the motor learning effect. 11 , 12 Numerous exoskeletons have been developed; however, only a few have been tailored for the pediatric population. Therefore, this study aimed to investigate the effects of overground RAGT using an untethered, torque-assisted, wearable exoskeletal robot in children with CP.

This multicenter, prospective, single-blinded randomized clinical trial was conducted from September 1, 2021, to March 31, 2023, at 5 pediatric rehabilitation centers in Korea. The internal review board of each participating hospital approved the study, and the trial was registered with the Clinical Research Information Service. The trial was reported in alignment with the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials ( CONSORT ) reporting guidelines. 13 Written informed consent was obtained from the patient’s parents. The flowchart of the study is shown in the Figure . The trial protocol can be found in Supplement 1 .

The study included children with CP aged 6 to 15 years who were 98 to 150 cm tall. Children with gait difficulty who walked with or without assistance at Gross Motor Function Classification System levels II to IV were included in the study. Children were excluded if they had severe intellectual disabilities that precluded understanding simple commands, severe lower limb contractures that interfered with wearing a robotic device, unhealed skin lesions on the lower limbs, a history of orthopedic surgery within the previous 12 months, or chemodenervation within the previous 6 months. Chemodenervation, surgery, or alteration of antispastic medication regimens were prohibited during the study. In total, 90 children were enrolled.

After the baseline assessments, participants were randomly assigned to the intervention or control group using a centralized web-based randomization system. An independent statistician generated a randomization list at the start of the trial using a computerized R program, version 3.5.1 (R Foundation). To avoid assessment bias, physical therapists blinded to the participants’ group assignments conducted all evaluations.

The experimental group underwent 30 minutes of RAGT, whereas the control group underwent 30 minutes of conventional physical therapy (PT) 3 days per week for 6 weeks. Both groups received the same total number of therapy sessions (18 sessions, 9 hours overall) during the intervention period. The number of training steps per session to assess training intensity was measured using a pedometer in the control group and a robotic device in the RAGT group.

The RAGT device was the Angel Legs M20, size S (Angel Robotics Inc), a powered lower-limb wearable robot designed to assist children with disabilities in improving their walking and lower limb mobility. Equipped with actuators at each hip and knee joint, the device can provide assistive torque according to the gait phase, automatically detected using the combined information from the ground contact sensor, encoders (incremental and absolute) in the actuators, and an inertial measurement unit sensor in a backpack (eFigure 1 in Supplement 2 ). 14 Robot-assisted gait training was performed overground in an indoor hospital setting, using an overhead lift or walker for balance, particularly for children with Gross Motor Function Classification System levels of III or IV, as needed. Each training session included the actual walking time in the RAGT (30 minutes), time to put on and take off the robot, rest, and evaluation of adverse events. The control group received conventional PT focused on gait training for 30 minutes per session in a 1-on-1 setting (eAppendix in Supplement 2 ). Participants who missed more than 10% of the treatment sessions within the 6-week intervention period were dropped from the study.

After the end of the intervention, follow-up assessments were conducted 4 weeks later to investigate the maintenance of the intervention effects. During this period, both groups received standard care, which consisted of a mean of 2 PT sessions weekly, including gait training, tone reduction, balance, and strengthening exercises. Therapies were individually adjusted based on the child’s function (eFigure 2 in Supplement 2 ).

Functional and kinematic assessments were performed for all patients at baseline (within 72 hours before the intervention), at the end of the 6-week intervention (within 1 week after intervention, posttest 1), and after the 4-week follow-up (22-34 days after intervention, posttest 2) to investigate effect maintenance.

The primary outcome measure was the overall gross motor function measured using the Gross Motor Function Measure (GMFM)-88. The GMFM-88 was selected as the primary outcome measure due to its robust validation and comprehensive ability to assess overall gross motor function changes, crucial for evaluating the impact of gait-focused interventions in CP. The percentage scores of dimensions D (standing ability) and E (walking, running, and climbing abilities), total GMFM-88 score, and GMFM-66 score were used for the analysis. The minimal clinically important difference (MCID) for the GMFM-88 total and GMFM-66 ranged from a 1.0% to 2.0% increase, suggesting a clinically significant improvement in gross motor function. 15 , 16

Balance control was assessed using the Pediatric Balance Scale, consisting of 14 items with 0 to 4 points per item and a maximum score of 56 points (with low scores indicating poor balance control and high scores indicating good balance control). 17 Selective voluntary motor control capacity was quantified using the Selective Control Assessment of the Lower Extremity (SCALE). 18 A SCALE score was obtained by summing the 0 to 2 points assigned to each of the 5 joints for a maximum of 10 points per limb. For both assessments, higher scores indicated better functioning.

To assess walking endurance and physical fitness, we measured walking distance (in meters) and oxygen consumption for 6 minutes (using the 6-minute walking test [6-minute WT]) with open-circuit spirometry (Kb4, 2 COSMED USA Inc). Subsequently, the oxygen rate and oxygen costs were calculated. The MCID for the 6-minute WT was 20 to 46 m. 15 Bioelectric impedance analysis was used to estimate muscle mass and body fat percentage as proportions of total body weight. The Pediatric Evaluation of Disability Inventory–Computer Adaptive Test (PEDI-CAT) was used to assess performance in daily activities. This test measures functional skills in 4 domains, including daily activity, mobility, social cognitive, and responsibility, with 276 items based on the parent or caregiver report. 19 In our study, the scaled scores (range, 0-100, with 0 indicating low functional ability and 100 indicating high functional ability) of each domain were used for the analysis.

The participants walked barefoot along a 10-m walkway at a self-selected speed. Participants were fitted with 16 passive reflective markers according to the Helen Hayes marker set. Kinematic and temporospatial data were measured using a computerized 3-dimensional motion analysis system (VICON MX-T10 Motion Analysis System, Oxford Metrics Inc). Spatiotemporal gait parameters, including cadence, walking speed, stride and step width, and single and double supports (percentage of the gait cycle) were used for analysis. The Gait Deviation Index (GDI) was calculated using kinematic data. The GDI quantifies overall gait pathology by providing a single score ranging from 0 to 100, with values closer to 100 indicating a normal gait pattern.

Sample size calculations were performed using a 2-tailed test with a randomized controlled design. A sample size of 82 was sufficient to detect an effect size of 0.6, power of 90%, and significance level of 5%. The sample size was increased to 90, allowing an anticipated dropout rate of approximately 10%.

A linear mixed-effects regression model was used to compare the efficacy of RAGT with that of conventional PT. We analyzed each outcome separately, with treatment, time, and interaction effects as independent variables. The Mann-Whitney U test or independent 2-tailed t test was used to compare the extent of improvement between the baseline and postintervention statuses. Mean differences at postintervention and after the 4-week follow-up were estimated by the group × time interaction term, with associated 95% CIs. P  < .025 indicated statistical significance by Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc analysis. Data were analyzed using SPSS, version 28 (IBM Inc).

Among 90 participants (mean [SD] age, 9.51 [2.48] years; 49 [54.4%] male and 41 [45.6%] female), 78 (86.7%) completed the intervention. Specifically, 37 participants (mean [SD] age, 9.57 [2.38] years; 18 [48.6%] female and 19 male [51.4%]) were randomly assigned to the RAGT group, and 41 participants (mean [SD] age, 9.32 [2.37] years; 15 [36.6%] female and 26 [63.4%] male) to the control group. Eight children in the RAGT group and 4 in the control group dropped out of the trial owing to consent withdrawal (n = 6) or SARS-CoV-2 infection of participants or parents (n = 6). Demographic characteristics did not differ significantly ( Table 1 ).

No safety issues were reported, and the experimental group experienced no adverse effects, such as skin lesions, pain, or fatigue, during RAGT. A specially trained physical therapist attached the RAGT device to all the participants. The number of training steps per session significantly differed between the groups (mean [SD] steps per session: RAGT group, 997.6 [124.5] steps; control group, 212.9 [116.6] steps; P  < .001).

The RAGT group showed significant improvements in gross motor function, as measured by the GMFM-88 total (mean difference, 2.64; 95% CI, 0.50-4.78), dimension E (mean difference, 2.70; 95% CI, 0.08-5.33), and GMFM-66 (mean difference, 2.52; 95% CI, 0.42-4.63) scores compared with the control group at the postintervention assessment. Furthermore, the immediate increases in the GMFM-88 total, dimension E, and GMFM-66 scores in the RAGT group in our study were 2.7%, 2.3%, and 1.2%, respectively, compared with baseline, for which the MCID thresholds were achieved after the intervention, indicating clinically significant changes. This improvement was maintained until the 4-week follow-up ( Table 2 ).

Improvement of balance control measured by the Pediatric Balance Scale was significantly greater in RAGT group compared with the control group at the 4-week follow-up (mean difference, 1.48; 95% CI, 0.03-2.94). Selective motor control measured using SCALE was improved in both groups, with no significant group differences.

The improvement in gait endurance on the 6-minute WT was observed only in the RAGT group at 4-week follow-up, with no significant differences between the groups ( Table 3 ). There were no significant changes in physical fitness measures, including oxygen consumption and muscle and fat mass via bioelectric impedance analysis after intervention in both groups. According to the PEDI-CAT, changes in the responsibility domain were greater in the RAGT group compared with the control group (mean difference, 2.52; 95% CI, 0.42-4.63). The changes in the daily activity (mean difference, 0.58; 95% CI, −0.30 to 1.46), mobility (mean difference, 0.10; 95% CI, −1.98 to 2.18), and social cognitive (mean difference, 0.01; 95% CI, −1.02 to 1.04) domains did not significantly differ between the groups ( Table 3 ).

Regarding the temporospatial parameters, decrement of step width was significantly greater in the RAGT group compared with the control group, indicating improvement (mean difference, −0.05; 95% CI, −0.08 to −0.01). However, gait speed and stride length did not significantly change ( Table 4 ). Notably, at the 4-week follow-up, the duration of single limb support of the more involved limb increased, whereas double limb support decreased only in the RAGT group, indicating improvement but without significant group differences. Improvement on the GDI was significantly greater in the RAGT group compared with the control group at 4-week follow-up (mean difference, 6.48; 95% CI, 2.77-10.19).

We reported the findings of a large, multicenter trial involving the use of a newly developed torque-assisted wearable robot in children with CP. After a 6-week RAGT training, gross motor function improved in the study group compared with the control group, and the effect was maintained throughout the follow-up. The immediate increases in the GMFM-88 total, dimension E, and GMFM-66 scores in the RAGT group in our study were 2.7%, 2.3%, and 1.2%, respectively, compared with baseline, for which the MCID thresholds were achieved after the intervention, indicating clinically significant changes. Gait pattern, balance control, and participation in daily living also improved after RAGT compared with the control group. No adverse events, such as pain, skin lesions, increased fatigue, or falls, were reported.

Robot-assisted gait training enhances motor function through repetitive and intensive training with a constant gait pattern. A certain threshold for repetition must be crossed to induce effective brain reorganization. This experience-dependent neuroplasticity requires at least 1000 repetitions of the same task to achieve permanent change at the synaptic level. 20 Herein, the RAGT group reached nearly 1000 steps per training session, 4.7 times the training intensity of the control group.

Although partial weight-bearing treadmill training has been proven effective, the evidence supporting the effectiveness of RAGT remains inconclusive. A recent meta-analysis revealed that RAGT did not confer significantly improved gross motor function, 6-minute WT performance, or gait speed compared with a dose-matched standard of care. 6 In another meta-analysis that compared RAGT with various control groups, 21 RAGT was associated with improved gait speed and gross motor function compared with conventional PT but was not superior to treadmill gait training or, when used in combination with conventional PT and RAGT, was not superior to PT alone. The heterogeneity of the RAGT effect may be explained by the following factors: device type, purpose of the device and interacting interface, study design, therapeutic dose with randomized clinical trial design, function of the participants, and outcome measurements.

Active engagement in training is critical for effective neurorehabilitation. During overground gait training, children can navigate and explore within a more natural environment, achieve active neuromuscular engagement, and attain greater stride-to-stride variability. 22 Additionally, overground RAGT exoskeletons seem more effective for dynamic balance than tethered robots, facilitating appropriate body alignment during weight-shifting movements. 14 , 23 The normal gait control mechanism involves maintaining stability in the anterior and lateral directions during forward progression. 24 Notably, the RAGT used in this study was able to capture step initiation using a ground contact and inertial measurement unit sensor to assist torque according to the gait phase. This intention-capturing feedback mechanism may elicit longer-term effects than passive training, which uses a bottom-up approach. In this study, balance improved in both groups; however, the carryover effect was maintained only in the RAGT group.

Passive rhythmic movement activates the gait center or central pattern generators in the spinal cord. 25 Compared with the traditional treadmill training or RAGT, this new overground RAGT offering variable gait experience might elicit a higher level of spine and brain neuroplasticity. 26 Recently, to overcome passive aspects and restricted movement variability of treadmill-tethered RAGT, some devices have offered varying levels of guidance or assistance that may be adjusted based on the patient’s requirements. Compared with treadmill training or end-effector-type RAGT, the RAGT exoskeleton enables more physiologic and reproducible sensorimotor experience in joint angular motion. These effects can be maintained while walking despite removing the device. In this study, gait pattern improved by increasing GDI and single-limb support and decreasing double-limb support. Robot-assisted gait training enables a sufficient stance phase on the side of the affected limb, promoting gait symmetry and reducing the compensatory mechanism during gait by assisting joint movements.

Predefined gait trajectory control is the most common assistive control strategy for exoskeletons. Trajectory-controlled RAGT devices are relatively simple but force the wearer to walk on a reference trajectory, possibly not aligning with their natural walking pattern. 27 Patients with complete paralysis or severe motor impairment can be trained to walk while wearing a robotic device, whereas movement can be too restricted in patients with incomplete paralysis. Robot-assisted gait training that provides continuous full guidance often makes patients passive and reduces their effort, weakening the motor learning effect in patients with incomplete paralysis, 11 , 12 whereas model-based control assists patients with mild to moderate paralysis but requires a precise dynamic model of the human exoskeleton system. 7 , 27 The unique feature of the RAGT device in this study is that the torque can be adjusted according to the patient’s residual muscle strength, allowing more active participation, variability of movements, and dynamic gait pattern adaptation. 12 This robot can offer assistance at various joints at varying levels, which enables customized intervention.

This study had several limitations. First, lower limb movements were assisted only in the sagittal plane. Second, only patients who could use a small RAGT device were included in this study. Additional studies are required to delineate the optimal training parameters and the role of the robot device type according to the patient profile.

In this randomized clinical trial, overground RAGT using a wearable robot significantly improved gross motor function, balance control, and gait pattern compared with a matched conventional PT group. This new torque-assisted, wearable RAGT benefits children with CP, supporting power-as-needed control, motivating children to explore walking, and providing intensive gait training. These benefits could also apply to children with prewalking motor abilities.

Accepted for Publication: May 17, 2024.

Published: July 22, 2024. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2024.22625

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License . © 2024 Choi JY et al. JAMA Network Open .

Corresponding Author: Min-Keun Song, MD, PhD, Department of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, Chonnam National University Medical School and Hospital, 160 Baekseo-ro Dong-gu, Gwangju, 61469, Republic of Korea ( [email protected] ).

Author Contributions: Profs Choi and Song had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Choi, Kim, H. Park, Song.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Choi, Hong.

Critical review of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Choi, Kim, H. Park, Yang, D. Park, Song.

Statistical analysis: Choi.

Obtained funding: Choi, Kim, H. Park, Song.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Choi, Hong, H. Park, Yang, D. Park.

Supervision: Choi, Yang.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This research was supported by a grant from the Intelligent Robot Supply and Expansion Project 2022 through the Ministry of Trade, Industry, and Energy of the Republic of Korea and Korea Institute for Robot Industry Advancement (Rehabilitation Robot Clinical Empirical Supported Program 2022; Prof Choi, principal investigator). This research was also supported by grant BCRI-23052 from Chonnam National University Hospital Biomedical Research Institute (Prof Song).

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 3 .

  • Register for email alerts with links to free full-text articles
  • Access PDFs of free articles
  • Manage your interests
  • Save searches and receive search alerts

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

land-logo

Article Menu

research abstract on child development

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Evaluating the quality of children’s active school travel spaces and the mechanisms of school district friendliness impact based on multi-source big data.

research abstract on child development

1. Introduction

2. literature review, 2.1. asts definition, 2.2. asts measurements, 2.3. child-friendly influences, 3. materials and methods, 3.1. study area and data acquisition, 3.2. evaluation of asts, 3.2.1. construction of walking routes network, 3.2.2. linear measurement of asts at the mesoscale, 3.2.3. node measurement of asts at the microscale, 3.2.4. comprehensive assessment of asts, 3.3. correlation analysis of route friendliness, 4. results and analysis, 4.1. walking routes network analysis, 4.1.1. linear characteristics of asts, 4.1.2. node characteristics of asts, 4.2. comprehensive evaluation of child friendliness, 4.2.1. spatial distribution characteristics, 4.2.2. classification and characteristics, 4.3. factors influencing friendliness, 5. discussion and conclusions, 5.1. a potential complement to asts measurements, 5.2. more effective ways to improve friendliness based on findings, 5.3. conclusions, author contributions, data availability statement, acknowledgments, conflicts of interest.

  • Hong, Q.; Lin, G.; Jie, Z. The Implement Paths of International Child-Friendly Cities and Their Enlightenment to Beijing. Urban Dev. Stud. 2022 , 29 , 1–5. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Guo, D.; Shi, Y.; Chen, R. Environmental Affordances and Children’s Needs: Insights from Child-Friendly Community Streets in China. Front. Archit. Res. 2023 , 12 , 411–422. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF). Child Friendly Cities and Communities Handbook ; United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF): Geneva, Switzerland, 2018. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang, J.; Hao, W.; Yi, Z. Research on Characteristics and Interactive Mechanism of Street Walkability Through Multi-source Big Data: Nanjing Central District as a Case Study. Urban Plan. Int. 2019 , 34 , 33–42. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lucas, C. How a Child-Friendly Lens Can Transform Urban Planning. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2021 , 5 , 472. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ikeda, E.; Oliver, M.; Witten, K.; Hinckson, E. Development in Measurement of Children’s Perceptions on Built Environment and Their School Travel Routes. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2017 , 20 , e121–e122. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Min, T.; Min, T.; Shouming, C.; Zhiyi, Z. Study on the Characteristics and Influencing Factors of Children’s Independent School Commuting in Chengdu. Community Des. 2023 , 138–143. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buttazzoni, A.N.; Coen, S.E.; Gilliland, J.A. Supporting Active School Travel: A Qualitative Analysis of Implementing a Regional Safe Routes to School Program. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018 , 212 , 181–190. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Wei, S.; Sang, L.; Cong, H.; Lei, M.J.; Zheng, X.X. Primary School Access Path Space Research: A Case Study of Wuhan Nanhu School District. Chin. Overseas Archit. 2023 , 4 , 85–89. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Leung, K.Y.K.; Loo, B.P.Y. Determinants of Children’s Active Travel to School: A Case Study in Hong Kong. Travel Behav. Soc. 2020 , 21 , 79–89. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Xing, H.; Huang, X.; Xia, Y. Associations of neighborhood and school built environment with active commuting to school in children: An empirical study of Xi’an City based on cross-classified multilevel model. J. Northwest Univ. (Nat. Sci. Ed.) 2023 , 53 , 749–760. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wang, X.; Liu, Y.; Yao, Y.; Zhou, S.; Zhu, Q.; Liu, M.; Helbich, M. Adolescents’ Environmental Perceptions Mediate Associations between Streetscape Environments and Active School Travel. Transp. Res. Part D Transp. Environ. 2023 , 114 , 103549. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Xiaoge, W.; DuoDuo, H.; Ye, L.; Yao, Y.; Lili, X. Impact of Human-Scale Street Space Quality on Walking to School by School-Age Children: A Case Study of Beijing. City Plan. Rev. 2023 . (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Xia, S.; Zhou, X.; Wu, Z. Design of Child-Friendly Routes to School. Urban Transp. China 2022 , 20 , 73–82. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Han, H.; Huang, J.; Jia, X.; Zhang, J.; Guo, J. Evaluation of Route Choice for Walking Commutes to School and Street Space Optimization in Old Urban Areas of China Based on a Child-friendly Orientation: The Case of the Wuyi Park Area in Zhengzhou. Child Adolesc. Soc. Work J. 2023 , 38 , 1527–1556. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bautista-Hernández, D.A. Determinants and Metropolitan Patterns of School Travel Time. Is Location Associated with Average Years of Schooling?: The Case of Mexico City. Cities 2023 , 139 , 104408. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Li, Y.; Yabuki, N.; Fukuda, T. Integrating GIS, Deep Learning, and Environmental Sensors for Multicriteria Evaluation of Urban Street Walkability. Landsc. Urban Plan 2023 , 230 , 104603. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Frank, L.D.; Sallis, J.F.; Saelens, B.E.; Leary, L.; Cain, K.; Conway, T.L.; Hess, P.M. The Development of a Walkability Index: Application to the Neighborhood Quality of Life Study. Br. J. Sports Med. 2010 , 44 , 924–933. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hall, C.M.; Ram, Y. Measuring the Relationship between Tourism and Walkability? Walk Score and English Tourist Attractions. J. Sustain. Tour. 2019 , 27 , 223–240. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yan, T.; Jin, Y.; Jin, H. Combined Effects of the Visual-Thermal Environment on the Subjective Evaluation of Urban Pedestrian Streets in Severely Cold Regions of China. Build. Environ. 2023 , 228 , 109895. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Liu, Y.; Yang, D.; Timmermans, H.J.P.; de Vries, B. Analysis of the Impact of Street-Scale Built Environment Design near Metro Stations on Pedestrian and Cyclist Road Segment Choice: A Stated Choice Experiment. J. Transp. Geogr. 2020 , 82 , 102570. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Nagata, S.; Nakaya, T.; Hanibuchi, T.; Amagasa, S.; Kikuchi, H.; Inoue, S. Objective Scoring of Streetscape Walkability Related to Leisure Walking: Statistical Modeling Approach with Semantic Segmentation of Google Street View Images. Health Place 2020 , 66 , 102428. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhao, L.; Xuan, W.; Yao, Y.; Amagasa, S.; Kikuchi, H.; Inoue, S. Study on the spatial correlation between urban walk ability and comprehensive vitality. Sci. Surv. Mapp. 2023 , 48 , 212–230. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wei, X.; Kang, P.; Chuanyun, Z. The Correlation Between Street Space and Behaviours in the New Data Environment. South Archit. 2023 , 4 , 49–60. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu, J.; Ettema, D.; Helbich, M. Street View Environments Are Associated with the Walking Duration of Pedestrians: The Case of Amsterdam, the Netherlands. Landsc. Urban Plan 2023 , 235 , 104752. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Fernández-Barrés, S.; Robinson, O.; Fossati, S.; Márquez, S.; Basagaña, X.; de Bont, J.; de Castro, M.; Donaire-Gonzalez, D.; Maitre, L.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.; et al. Urban Environment and Health Behaviours in Children from Six European Countries. Environ. Int. 2022 , 165 , 107319. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Rothman, L.; Hagel, B.; Howard, A.; Cloutier, M.S.; Macpherson, A.; Aguirre, A.N.; McCormack, G.R.; Fuselli, P.; Buliung, R.; HubkaRao, T.; et al. Active School Transportation and the Built Environment across Canadian Cities: Findings from the Child Active Transportation Safety and the Environment (CHASE) Study. Prev. Med. 2021 , 146 , 106470. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Giles-Corti, B.; Wood, G.; Pikora, T.; Learnihan, V.; Bulsara, M.; Van Niel, K.; Timperio, A.; McCormack, G.; Villanueva, K. School Site and the Potential to Walk to School: The Impact of Street Connectivity and Traffic Exposure in School Neighborhoods. Health Place 2011 , 17 , 545–550. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Su, J.G.; Jerrett, M.; McConnell, R.; Berhane, K.; Dunton, G.; Shankardass, K.; Reynolds, K.; Chang, R.; Wolch, J. Factors Influencing Whether Children Walk to School. Health Place 2013 , 22 , 153–161. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Li, M.; Wang, Y.; Zhou, D. Effects of the Built Environment and Sociodemographic Characteristics on Children’s School Travel. Transp. Policy 2023 , 134 , 191–202. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Nan, F. Policy Innovation on Building Child Friendly Cities in China: Evidence from Four Chinese Cities. Child. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2020 , 118 , 105491. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Molina-García, J.; Campos, S.; García-Massó, X.; Herrador-Colmenero, M.; Gálvez-Fernández, P.; Molina-Soberanes, D.; Queralt, A.; Chillón, P. Different Neighborhood Walkability Indexes for Active Commuting to School Are Necessary for Urban and Rural Children and Adolescents. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2020 , 17 , 124. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Anthopoulos, L.; Kazantzi, V. Urban Energy Efficiency Assessment Models from an AI and Big Data Perspective: Tools for Policy Makers. Sustain. Cities Soc. 2022 , 76 , 103492. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Li, W.; Chen, M.; Yao, N.; Luo, Z.; Jiao, Y. Spatial-Temporal Evolution of Roadway Layout System from a Space Syntax Perspective. Tunn. Undergr. Space Technol 2023 , 135 , 105038. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wei, G.; Menghan, J.; Mei, Z.; Yon, G.; Haohan, M. Review of Progress and Quantitative Measurement Methods of Research on Street Space. City Plan. Rev. 2022 , 46 , 106–114. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cordts, M.; Omran, M.; Ramos, S.; Rehfeld, T.; Enzweiler, M.; Benenson, R.; Franke, U.; Roth, S.; Schiele, B. The Cityscapes Dataset for Semantic Urban Scene Understanding. In Proceedings of the 2016 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR), Las Vegas, NV, USA, 27–30 June 2016; pp. 3213–3223. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duffy, R.T.; Larsen, K.; Bélanger, M.; Brussoni, M.; Faulkner, G.; Gunnell, K.; Tremblay, M.S.; Larouche, R. Children’s Independent Mobility, School Travel, and the Surrounding Neighborhood. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2024 , 66 , 819–831. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Huo, H.; Li, Z.; Wang, X. Study on the Evaluation and Optimization of School Commuting Space Surrounding Primary Schools in Shijiazhuang. Urban. Archit. 2021 , 18 , 92–95. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wang,, F.; Mao, A.; Li, H.; Jia, M. Quality Measurement and Regional Difference of Urbanization in Shandong Province Based on the Entropy Method. Sci. Geogr. Sin. 2013 , 33 , 1323–1329. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Queralt, A.; Molina-García, J. Physical Activity and Active Commuting in Relation to Objectively Measured Built-Environment Attributes among Adolescents. J. Phys. Act. Health 2019 , 16 , 371–374. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ulfarsson, G.F.; Shankar, V.N. Children’s Travel to School: Discrete Choice Modeling of Correlated Motorized and Nonmotorized Transportation Modes Using Covariance Heterogeneity. Environ. Plan. B Urban Anal. City Sci. 2008 , 35 , 195–206. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kang, C.-D. Measuring the Effects of Street Network Configurations on Walking in Seoul, Korea. Cities 2017 , 71 , 30–40. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Basu, R.; Sevtsuk, A. How Do Street Attributes Affect Willingness-to-Walk? City-Wide Pedestrian Route Choice Analysis Using Big Data from Boston and San Francisco. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pr. 2022 , 163 , 1–19. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Khanian, M.; Łaszkiewicz, E.; Kronenberg, J. Exposure to Greenery during Children’s Home–School Walks: Socio-Economic Inequalities in Alternative Routes. Transp. Res. Part D Transp. Env. 2024 , 130 , 104162. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ikeda, E.; Mavoa, S.; Hinckson, E.; Witten, K.; Donnellan, N.; Smith, M. Differences in Child-Drawn and GIS-Modelled Routes to School: Impact on Space and Exposure to the Built Environment in Auckland, New Zealand. J. Transp. Geogr. 2018 , 71 , 103–115. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ivan, L.; Dikken, J.; Van Hoof, J. Unveiling the Experienced Age-Friendliness of Older People in Bucharest: A Comprehensive Study Using the Validated Romanian Age-Friendly Cities and Communities Questionnaire and Cluster Analysis. Habitat Int. 2024 , 143 , 102973. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhen, X.; Anqi, L.; Zhenqi, Z.; Linyun, H. Analyzing and Scenarioizing Walking Routes and Urban Context of Middle School Students with Baidu Map: A Case Study of Jianye District of Nanjing. Mod. Urban Res. 2021 , 33–40 , 91. (In Chinese) [ Google Scholar ]

Click here to enlarge figure

System LayerCriteria LayerIndicator LayerMeaning of VariablesMeasurement of VariablesWeight
Accessibility (0.301)Road accessibilityConnectivityNumber of connections to other roads for this sectionSum of connectivity values for each segment of roads between school and residential area0.033
ChoiceDifficulty level of selection for this Section in the entire road networkSum of selection degree values for each segment of roads between school and residential area0.031
IntegrationDifficulty level of reaching this section in the entire road networkSum of Integration degree values for each segment of roads between school and residential area0.063
Schoolfront space accessibilityRoad congestionCongestion within 200 m range of the school gate during peak school hoursUsing Amap road condition prediction module, rate smoothness as follows: smooth = 4, good = 3, congested = 2, very congested = 10.087
Number of IntersectionsNumber of intersections within 100 m of the school frontUse ArcGIS to make a 100 m buffer with the school entrance and count the number of intersections in the buffer.0.039
Relative pedestrian widthActual utilized width of the straight pedestrian walkway in front of the schoolCalculate the percentage of remaining effective width after objects such as transformer boxes, utility poles, and vehicles occupy the roadway, relative to the total road width0.048
Safety (0.357)Road safetyPedestrian space continuity *Continuity of pedestrian walkways in the commuting pathCalculate the percentage of the total path length after removing the length of crosswalks, underground passages, pedestrian overpasses, and stairs from the road path0.070
Pedestrian crossing facilities *Number of pedestrian crossing facilities in the commuting pathCount the number of pedestrian crossing facilities, assign different scores to different types: underground passages = 2, stairs = 1, crosswalks = −10.065
Number of turns *Number of turns in the commuting pathCount the number of direction changes in the commuting path0.080
Schoolfront space safetySchoolfront roads *Road grade of the road facing the school gateUsing OSM data, classify schoolfront roads into: side roads = 3, minor roads = 2, main Roads = 10.051
Traffic calming measuresTraffic calming facilities at the school gateBased on investigative,
Evaluate the quantity of design elements aimed at reducing vehicle speed, including pavement (color, guide lines, crosswalks, etc.), facilities (speed bumps, signs, etc.), and elevation changes (curbs, flower beds, etc.).good = 4 (>6), suitable = 3 (5–6), average = 2 (3–4), poor = 1 (≤2)
0.037
School gate buffer zoneSize of the buffer zone at the school gateBased on investigative, rate the buffer zone area: adequate = 4, suitable = 3, average = 2, poor = 10.053
Comfort (0.342)Road comfortCommuting distance *Actual distance of the commuting pathCalculate the actual distance of the commuting Path0.100
Depth *Minimum number of times needed to reach other sections from this sectionSum of depth values for each segment of roads between school and residential area0.039
Detour ratio *Detour level of the commuting pathRatio of actual commuting path length to Euclidean distance0.053
Schoolfront space comfortInterface diversityTypes and quantity of visual elements at the school gateBased on investigative, rate the appearance of decorative signs, exhibition boards, etc.: suitable = 3, average = 2, poor = 10.020
Waiting spaceArea for parents waiting for students at the school gateBased on investigative, score the size of the waiting space for pick-ups and drop-offs and the facilities available for resting, such as benches: suitable = 3, average = 2, poor = 10.109
Parking organizationTemporary parking organization at the school gateRate the temporary parking area: adequate = 4, suitable = 3, exists but insufficient = 2, none = 10.021
Road AccessibilitySchoolfront Space AccessibilityRoad SafetySchoolfront Space SafetyRoad ComfortSchoolfront Space ComfortWeight
Road accessibility-21214321300.098
Schoolfront space accessibility86-346434510.193
Road safety8651-6439600.215
Schoolfront space safety433030-26430.123
Road comfort86514773-640.230
Schoolfront space comfort6034304330-0.141
Influencing FactorVariableDefinitionUnitsMeanStandard Deviation
Built environmentX1 School district Scale *School district areakm 0.9111.134
X2 School Centrality *Straight-Line distance between school location and geometric centerkm0.6880.308
X3 Environmental ComfortRatio of green area to school district area%34.4304.237
X4 Transportation ConvenienceRatio of road length to school district areakm/km 6.3911.489
X5 Land Use DiversityMixed status of POI within school district-0.8730.620
X6 Land Development IntensityRatio of building footprint area to school district area%9.1083.675
X7 School Frontage AreaStreet view image recognitionm 139.2302.421
Social environmentX8 Economic IntensityAverage housing price within school district10,000 RMB/m 0.9780.465
X9 Population DensityAverage population within school district10,000 person/km 0.3620.289
X10 AgglomerationCold-Hot Spot analysis calculation in ArcGIS-1.0130.324
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Lu, C.; Yu, C.; Liu, X. Evaluating the Quality of Children’s Active School Travel Spaces and the Mechanisms of School District Friendliness Impact Based on Multi-Source Big Data. Land 2024 , 13 , 1319. https://doi.org/10.3390/land13081319

Lu C, Yu C, Liu X. Evaluating the Quality of Children’s Active School Travel Spaces and the Mechanisms of School District Friendliness Impact Based on Multi-Source Big Data. Land . 2024; 13(8):1319. https://doi.org/10.3390/land13081319

Lu, Chenyu, Changbin Yu, and Xiaowan Liu. 2024. "Evaluating the Quality of Children’s Active School Travel Spaces and the Mechanisms of School District Friendliness Impact Based on Multi-Source Big Data" Land 13, no. 8: 1319. https://doi.org/10.3390/land13081319

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Research in Early Childhood Development: Selected Abstracts

    research abstract on child development

  2. (PDF) Nature-Based Early Childhood Education and Children’s Social

    research abstract on child development

  3. Intro to child development

    research abstract on child development

  4. Child development

    research abstract on child development

  5. (PDF) Research on Child and Adolescent Development and Public Policy in

    research abstract on child development

  6. Whole child development: Measuring what matters

    research abstract on child development

COMMENTS

  1. A STUDY ON CHILDHOOD DEVELOPMENT IN EARLY STAGE

    Early Childhood Development refers to the physical, cognitive, linguistic, and socio-emotional. development of a child from the prenatal stage up to age eight. This development happens in a ...

  2. Parenting and Child Development: A Relational Health Perspective

    Abstract. A child's development is embedded within a complex system of relationships. Among the many relationships that influence children's growth and development, perhaps the most influential is the one that exists between parent and child. Recognition of the critical importance of early parent-child relationship quality for children's ...

  3. Early childhood development: an imperative for action and measurement

    It is estimated that, in 2010, at least 249 million (43%) children under the age of 5 years in low-income and middle-income countries (LMICs) were at risk of poor early childhood development (ECD) as a consequence of being stunted or living in extreme poverty. 7 This loss of potential is costly for individuals and societies.

  4. Child Development in a Changing World: Risks and Opportunities

    Abstract. This review explores current understandings of child development and the consequences for children of risk exposure in low- and middle-income countries by integrating empirical evidence from development economics with insights from allied social science disciplines.

  5. Child Development

    As the flagship journal of the Society for Research in Child Development, Child Development has published articles, essays, reviews, and tutorials on various topics in the field of child development since 1930. Spanning many disciplines, the journal provides the latest research, not only for researchers and theoreticians, but also for child psychiatrists, clinical psychologists, psychiatric ...

  6. (PDF) Child and Adolescent Development

    These developmental stages include the. sensorimotor (0-2 years), preoperational (2-7 years), concrete. operational (7-11 years), and formal operational (11 years+) periods. To today, the stages ...

  7. Child Development

    Child Development, the flagship journal of the Society for Research in Child Development, has published articles, essays, reviews, and tutorials on various topics in the field of Child Development for almost 100 years. We have a wide readership including researchers, theoreticians, child psychiatrists, clinical psychologists, psychiatric social workers, specialists in early childhood education ...

  8. <em>Child Development</em>

    For most children, the family is the primary educational environment that lays the foundation for their development (Landry et al., 2006).This includes children's cognitive ability, which can be defined as a general mental capability involving reasoning, planning, problem-solving, abstract thinking, complex idea comprehension, and learning from experience (Gottfredson, 1997).

  9. Journal of Early Childhood Research: Sage Journals

    The Journal of Early Childhood Research is a peer-reviewed journal that provides an international forum for childhood research, bridging cross-disciplinary areas and applying theory and research within the professional community. This reflects the world-wide growth in theoretical and empirical research on learning and development in early childhood and the impact of this on provision.

  10. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development is a series of extended works that present in-depth research studies and significant findings in child development. Abstract Scientists have, for some time, recognized that development unfolds in numerous settings, including families, schools, neighborhoods, and organized and ...

  11. Infant and Child Development

    Infant and Child Development: prenatal, childhood, adolescence, emerging adulthood is a premier developmental science journal for transparent and open research. ... Qualitative Research on Infant and Child Development in Majority World Countries. Abstract Deadline (Optional) - 31 July 2024 Submission Deadline - 31 October 2024 . Guest Editor ...

  12. Child Development

    Child Development. Since its inception in 1930, Child Development has been devoted to original contributions on topics in child development from the fetal period through adolescence. It is a vital source of information not only for researchers and theoreticians, but for a broad range of psychiatrists and psychologists, educators, and social ...

  13. PDF Child and Adolescent Development Research and Teacher Education ...

    Child and Adolescent Development Research and Teacher Education: Evidence-based Pedagogy, Policy, and Practice Summary of Roundtable Meetings . December 1-2, 2005 . March 20-21, 2006 . Co-Sponsored by: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) National Institutes of Health . U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES . and

  14. Parenting and Child Development: A Relational Health Perspective

    Among the many relationships that influence children's growth and development, perhaps the most influential is the one that exists between parent and child. Recognition of the critical importance of early parent-child relationship quality for children's socioemotional, cognitive, neurobiological, and health outcomes has contributed to a ...

  15. Child Development and Early Learning

    The domains of child development and early learning are discussed in different terms and categorized in different ways in the various fields and disciplines that are involved in research, practice, and policy related to children from birth through age 8. To organize the discussion in this report, the committee elected to use the approach and overarching terms depicted in Figure 4-1.

  16. Welcome to SRCD

    Publications. The Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD) publishes three journals, as well as the Social Policy Report. SRCD members receive full digital access to all three journals as a benefit of membership. View our publications.

  17. Child Development Submission Guidelines

    Double-spacing throughout (abstract, body text, references) Using 12-point, Times New Roman font; 1-inch margins; ... covers the full range of substantive issues in child development research, (c) using a wide variety of methods, and (d) submitted by authors who represent a cross-section of the SRCD and child development research more broadly ...

  18. Maternal sensitivity, early interactions and infant attachment: a

    Research: Early interactions and infant attachment, early intervention, social and biological risks infants, prevention and promotion infant development programs, Strange Situation Procedure-SSP (Certificate of Reliability by the University of Minnesota (2017), Spanish adaptation of the 4th edition of the Bayley Scales of Child Development.

  19. (PDF) Implementing the zone of proximal development: From the

    Following The Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD) the teacher helped the children in the process of "making meaning learning" It followed the stage that by the help of adult (teacher, parents ...

  20. (PDF) Child & Adolescent Development

    Younger adolescents (11-15 years old) undergo rapid change, marked by increased curiosity, connections outside the family, and metacognition (Caskey & Anfara, 2014). Older adolescents (15-25 years ...

  21. A longitudinal study of the relationship between children's exposure to

    Aim: This study addresses the scarcity of longitudinal research on the influence of screen media on children. It aims to explore the longitudinal relationship between children's vocabulary development and their exposure to screen media. Methods: The study, initiated in 2017, included 72 children (37 boys) in Östergötland, Sweden, at three key developmental stages: preverbal (9.7 months ...

  22. Publishing child development research from around the world: An unfair

    Publishing scientific research in high-impact journals is not easy—in any field, or from any setting. However, it has become increasingly apparent to the diverse set of co-authors of this paper (for author positionality statements, see Appendix A) that publishing research on child development from certain countries is especially challenging.These countries have been referred to collectively ...

  23. Using parent report to measure vocabulary in young bilingual children

    A large number of children are exposed to more than one language. One well‐established method of assessing early vocabulary development in monolingual children is parent report; however, its use in bilingual/multilingual contexts is less established and brings unique challenges. In this methodological scoping review, we reviewed studies of early vocabulary development using parent report ...

  24. The economics of human development: 'Investing in children' or

    The notion of investing in children is not new to HCT - the works of Theodore Shultz (e.g., Schultz, 1973, 1974) and Gary Becker (e.g., Becker, 1993, 1994) are a testament to that.However, as Kilburn and Karoly (2008) put it, the (re)new(ed) attention devoted by the 'dismal science' and 'Nobel-Prize winning economists' (Kilburn and Karoly, 2008: 1) to children and investments in ...

  25. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    The Science of Learning and Teaching at Home: An SRCD Virtual Issue. The Society for Research in Child Development is pleased to share a cross journal virtual issue on The Science of Learning and Teaching at Home.These articles, freely available online until July 30, 2020, were selected from SRCD's scholarly journals and publications because they explore research relevant to remote learning ...

  26. Maternal Urinary Fluoride and Child Neurobehavior at Age 36 Months

    Other studies conducted in Canada and Mexico have found associations of higher prenatal fluoride exposure at US-population-relevant levels with poorer neurocognitive outcomes, including lower IQ. 8-10,12,14,21 For example, a study conducted in the ELEMENT cohort found that each 0.5 mg/L increase in creatine-adjusted MUF was associated with a ...

  27. The importance of socializing children

    Facilitate opportunities for children to be themselves. Every child is different; one might enjoy playing sports, while another might play Dungeons and Dragons. Personalize ways they can engage with others. Encourage variety and risk taking. Making sure your child gets out of their comfort zone in a safe fashion is helpful. Join groups. Group ...

  28. Wearable Robot Gait Training in Children With Cerebral Palsy

    Importance Cerebral palsy (CP) is the most common developmental motor disorder in children. Robot-assisted gait training (RAGT) using a wearable robot can provide intensive overground walking experience. Objective To investigate the effectiveness of overground RAGT in children with CP using an untethered, torque-assisted, wearable exoskeletal robot.

  29. Land

    With the advancement of child-friendly urban planning initiatives, the significance of Active School Travel Spaces (ASTSs) in shaping urban development and promoting the physical and mental well-being of children has become increasingly apparent. This research focuses on 151 public primary schools in the central urban area of Lanzhou City. Utilizing the Amap pedestrian route planning API, we ...

  30. Subcortical and cerebellar volume differences in bilingual and

    Research suggests that bilingual children experience an extension or delay in the closing of the sensitive/critical period of language development due to multiple language exposure. Moreover, bilingual experience may impact the development of subcortical regions, although these conclusions are drawn from research with adults, as there is a scarcity of research during late childhood and early ...