19+ Experimental Design Examples (Methods + Types)

practical psychology logo

Ever wondered how scientists discover new medicines, psychologists learn about behavior, or even how marketers figure out what kind of ads you like? Well, they all have something in common: they use a special plan or recipe called an "experimental design."

Imagine you're baking cookies. You can't just throw random amounts of flour, sugar, and chocolate chips into a bowl and hope for the best. You follow a recipe, right? Scientists and researchers do something similar. They follow a "recipe" called an experimental design to make sure their experiments are set up in a way that the answers they find are meaningful and reliable.

Experimental design is the roadmap researchers use to answer questions. It's a set of rules and steps that researchers follow to collect information, or "data," in a way that is fair, accurate, and makes sense.

experimental design test tubes

Long ago, people didn't have detailed game plans for experiments. They often just tried things out and saw what happened. But over time, people got smarter about this. They started creating structured plans—what we now call experimental designs—to get clearer, more trustworthy answers to their questions.

In this article, we'll take you on a journey through the world of experimental designs. We'll talk about the different types, or "flavors," of experimental designs, where they're used, and even give you a peek into how they came to be.

What Is Experimental Design?

Alright, before we dive into the different types of experimental designs, let's get crystal clear on what experimental design actually is.

Imagine you're a detective trying to solve a mystery. You need clues, right? Well, in the world of research, experimental design is like the roadmap that helps you find those clues. It's like the game plan in sports or the blueprint when you're building a house. Just like you wouldn't start building without a good blueprint, researchers won't start their studies without a strong experimental design.

So, why do we need experimental design? Think about baking a cake. If you toss ingredients into a bowl without measuring, you'll end up with a mess instead of a tasty dessert.

Similarly, in research, if you don't have a solid plan, you might get confusing or incorrect results. A good experimental design helps you ask the right questions ( think critically ), decide what to measure ( come up with an idea ), and figure out how to measure it (test it). It also helps you consider things that might mess up your results, like outside influences you hadn't thought of.

For example, let's say you want to find out if listening to music helps people focus better. Your experimental design would help you decide things like: Who are you going to test? What kind of music will you use? How will you measure focus? And, importantly, how will you make sure that it's really the music affecting focus and not something else, like the time of day or whether someone had a good breakfast?

In short, experimental design is the master plan that guides researchers through the process of collecting data, so they can answer questions in the most reliable way possible. It's like the GPS for the journey of discovery!

History of Experimental Design

Around 350 BCE, people like Aristotle were trying to figure out how the world works, but they mostly just thought really hard about things. They didn't test their ideas much. So while they were super smart, their methods weren't always the best for finding out the truth.

Fast forward to the Renaissance (14th to 17th centuries), a time of big changes and lots of curiosity. People like Galileo started to experiment by actually doing tests, like rolling balls down inclined planes to study motion. Galileo's work was cool because he combined thinking with doing. He'd have an idea, test it, look at the results, and then think some more. This approach was a lot more reliable than just sitting around and thinking.

Now, let's zoom ahead to the 18th and 19th centuries. This is when people like Francis Galton, an English polymath, started to get really systematic about experimentation. Galton was obsessed with measuring things. Seriously, he even tried to measure how good-looking people were ! His work helped create the foundations for a more organized approach to experiments.

Next stop: the early 20th century. Enter Ronald A. Fisher , a brilliant British statistician. Fisher was a game-changer. He came up with ideas that are like the bread and butter of modern experimental design.

Fisher invented the concept of the " control group "—that's a group of people or things that don't get the treatment you're testing, so you can compare them to those who do. He also stressed the importance of " randomization ," which means assigning people or things to different groups by chance, like drawing names out of a hat. This makes sure the experiment is fair and the results are trustworthy.

Around the same time, American psychologists like John B. Watson and B.F. Skinner were developing " behaviorism ." They focused on studying things that they could directly observe and measure, like actions and reactions.

Skinner even built boxes—called Skinner Boxes —to test how animals like pigeons and rats learn. Their work helped shape how psychologists design experiments today. Watson performed a very controversial experiment called The Little Albert experiment that helped describe behaviour through conditioning—in other words, how people learn to behave the way they do.

In the later part of the 20th century and into our time, computers have totally shaken things up. Researchers now use super powerful software to help design their experiments and crunch the numbers.

With computers, they can simulate complex experiments before they even start, which helps them predict what might happen. This is especially helpful in fields like medicine, where getting things right can be a matter of life and death.

Also, did you know that experimental designs aren't just for scientists in labs? They're used by people in all sorts of jobs, like marketing, education, and even video game design! Yes, someone probably ran an experiment to figure out what makes a game super fun to play.

So there you have it—a quick tour through the history of experimental design, from Aristotle's deep thoughts to Fisher's groundbreaking ideas, and all the way to today's computer-powered research. These designs are the recipes that help people from all walks of life find answers to their big questions.

Key Terms in Experimental Design

Before we dig into the different types of experimental designs, let's get comfy with some key terms. Understanding these terms will make it easier for us to explore the various types of experimental designs that researchers use to answer their big questions.

Independent Variable : This is what you change or control in your experiment to see what effect it has. Think of it as the "cause" in a cause-and-effect relationship. For example, if you're studying whether different types of music help people focus, the kind of music is the independent variable.

Dependent Variable : This is what you're measuring to see the effect of your independent variable. In our music and focus experiment, how well people focus is the dependent variable—it's what "depends" on the kind of music played.

Control Group : This is a group of people who don't get the special treatment or change you're testing. They help you see what happens when the independent variable is not applied. If you're testing whether a new medicine works, the control group would take a fake pill, called a placebo , instead of the real medicine.

Experimental Group : This is the group that gets the special treatment or change you're interested in. Going back to our medicine example, this group would get the actual medicine to see if it has any effect.

Randomization : This is like shaking things up in a fair way. You randomly put people into the control or experimental group so that each group is a good mix of different kinds of people. This helps make the results more reliable.

Sample : This is the group of people you're studying. They're a "sample" of a larger group that you're interested in. For instance, if you want to know how teenagers feel about a new video game, you might study a sample of 100 teenagers.

Bias : This is anything that might tilt your experiment one way or another without you realizing it. Like if you're testing a new kind of dog food and you only test it on poodles, that could create a bias because maybe poodles just really like that food and other breeds don't.

Data : This is the information you collect during the experiment. It's like the treasure you find on your journey of discovery!

Replication : This means doing the experiment more than once to make sure your findings hold up. It's like double-checking your answers on a test.

Hypothesis : This is your educated guess about what will happen in the experiment. It's like predicting the end of a movie based on the first half.

Steps of Experimental Design

Alright, let's say you're all fired up and ready to run your own experiment. Cool! But where do you start? Well, designing an experiment is a bit like planning a road trip. There are some key steps you've got to take to make sure you reach your destination. Let's break it down:

  • Ask a Question : Before you hit the road, you've got to know where you're going. Same with experiments. You start with a question you want to answer, like "Does eating breakfast really make you do better in school?"
  • Do Some Homework : Before you pack your bags, you look up the best places to visit, right? In science, this means reading up on what other people have already discovered about your topic.
  • Form a Hypothesis : This is your educated guess about what you think will happen. It's like saying, "I bet this route will get us there faster."
  • Plan the Details : Now you decide what kind of car you're driving (your experimental design), who's coming with you (your sample), and what snacks to bring (your variables).
  • Randomization : Remember, this is like shuffling a deck of cards. You want to mix up who goes into your control and experimental groups to make sure it's a fair test.
  • Run the Experiment : Finally, the rubber hits the road! You carry out your plan, making sure to collect your data carefully.
  • Analyze the Data : Once the trip's over, you look at your photos and decide which ones are keepers. In science, this means looking at your data to see what it tells you.
  • Draw Conclusions : Based on your data, did you find an answer to your question? This is like saying, "Yep, that route was faster," or "Nope, we hit a ton of traffic."
  • Share Your Findings : After a great trip, you want to tell everyone about it, right? Scientists do the same by publishing their results so others can learn from them.
  • Do It Again? : Sometimes one road trip just isn't enough. In the same way, scientists often repeat their experiments to make sure their findings are solid.

So there you have it! Those are the basic steps you need to follow when you're designing an experiment. Each step helps make sure that you're setting up a fair and reliable way to find answers to your big questions.

Let's get into examples of experimental designs.

1) True Experimental Design

notepad

In the world of experiments, the True Experimental Design is like the superstar quarterback everyone talks about. Born out of the early 20th-century work of statisticians like Ronald A. Fisher, this design is all about control, precision, and reliability.

Researchers carefully pick an independent variable to manipulate (remember, that's the thing they're changing on purpose) and measure the dependent variable (the effect they're studying). Then comes the magic trick—randomization. By randomly putting participants into either the control or experimental group, scientists make sure their experiment is as fair as possible.

No sneaky biases here!

True Experimental Design Pros

The pros of True Experimental Design are like the perks of a VIP ticket at a concert: you get the best and most trustworthy results. Because everything is controlled and randomized, you can feel pretty confident that the results aren't just a fluke.

True Experimental Design Cons

However, there's a catch. Sometimes, it's really tough to set up these experiments in a real-world situation. Imagine trying to control every single detail of your day, from the food you eat to the air you breathe. Not so easy, right?

True Experimental Design Uses

The fields that get the most out of True Experimental Designs are those that need super reliable results, like medical research.

When scientists were developing COVID-19 vaccines, they used this design to run clinical trials. They had control groups that received a placebo (a harmless substance with no effect) and experimental groups that got the actual vaccine. Then they measured how many people in each group got sick. By comparing the two, they could say, "Yep, this vaccine works!"

So next time you read about a groundbreaking discovery in medicine or technology, chances are a True Experimental Design was the VIP behind the scenes, making sure everything was on point. It's been the go-to for rigorous scientific inquiry for nearly a century, and it's not stepping off the stage anytime soon.

2) Quasi-Experimental Design

So, let's talk about the Quasi-Experimental Design. Think of this one as the cool cousin of True Experimental Design. It wants to be just like its famous relative, but it's a bit more laid-back and flexible. You'll find quasi-experimental designs when it's tricky to set up a full-blown True Experimental Design with all the bells and whistles.

Quasi-experiments still play with an independent variable, just like their stricter cousins. The big difference? They don't use randomization. It's like wanting to divide a bag of jelly beans equally between your friends, but you can't quite do it perfectly.

In real life, it's often not possible or ethical to randomly assign people to different groups, especially when dealing with sensitive topics like education or social issues. And that's where quasi-experiments come in.

Quasi-Experimental Design Pros

Even though they lack full randomization, quasi-experimental designs are like the Swiss Army knives of research: versatile and practical. They're especially popular in fields like education, sociology, and public policy.

For instance, when researchers wanted to figure out if the Head Start program , aimed at giving young kids a "head start" in school, was effective, they used a quasi-experimental design. They couldn't randomly assign kids to go or not go to preschool, but they could compare kids who did with kids who didn't.

Quasi-Experimental Design Cons

Of course, quasi-experiments come with their own bag of pros and cons. On the plus side, they're easier to set up and often cheaper than true experiments. But the flip side is that they're not as rock-solid in their conclusions. Because the groups aren't randomly assigned, there's always that little voice saying, "Hey, are we missing something here?"

Quasi-Experimental Design Uses

Quasi-Experimental Design gained traction in the mid-20th century. Researchers were grappling with real-world problems that didn't fit neatly into a laboratory setting. Plus, as society became more aware of ethical considerations, the need for flexible designs increased. So, the quasi-experimental approach was like a breath of fresh air for scientists wanting to study complex issues without a laundry list of restrictions.

In short, if True Experimental Design is the superstar quarterback, Quasi-Experimental Design is the versatile player who can adapt and still make significant contributions to the game.

3) Pre-Experimental Design

Now, let's talk about the Pre-Experimental Design. Imagine it as the beginner's skateboard you get before you try out for all the cool tricks. It has wheels, it rolls, but it's not built for the professional skatepark.

Similarly, pre-experimental designs give researchers a starting point. They let you dip your toes in the water of scientific research without diving in head-first.

So, what's the deal with pre-experimental designs?

Pre-Experimental Designs are the basic, no-frills versions of experiments. Researchers still mess around with an independent variable and measure a dependent variable, but they skip over the whole randomization thing and often don't even have a control group.

It's like baking a cake but forgetting the frosting and sprinkles; you'll get some results, but they might not be as complete or reliable as you'd like.

Pre-Experimental Design Pros

Why use such a simple setup? Because sometimes, you just need to get the ball rolling. Pre-experimental designs are great for quick-and-dirty research when you're short on time or resources. They give you a rough idea of what's happening, which you can use to plan more detailed studies later.

A good example of this is early studies on the effects of screen time on kids. Researchers couldn't control every aspect of a child's life, but they could easily ask parents to track how much time their kids spent in front of screens and then look for trends in behavior or school performance.

Pre-Experimental Design Cons

But here's the catch: pre-experimental designs are like that first draft of an essay. It helps you get your ideas down, but you wouldn't want to turn it in for a grade. Because these designs lack the rigorous structure of true or quasi-experimental setups, they can't give you rock-solid conclusions. They're more like clues or signposts pointing you in a certain direction.

Pre-Experimental Design Uses

This type of design became popular in the early stages of various scientific fields. Researchers used them to scratch the surface of a topic, generate some initial data, and then decide if it's worth exploring further. In other words, pre-experimental designs were the stepping stones that led to more complex, thorough investigations.

So, while Pre-Experimental Design may not be the star player on the team, it's like the practice squad that helps everyone get better. It's the starting point that can lead to bigger and better things.

4) Factorial Design

Now, buckle up, because we're moving into the world of Factorial Design, the multi-tasker of the experimental universe.

Imagine juggling not just one, but multiple balls in the air—that's what researchers do in a factorial design.

In Factorial Design, researchers are not satisfied with just studying one independent variable. Nope, they want to study two or more at the same time to see how they interact.

It's like cooking with several spices to see how they blend together to create unique flavors.

Factorial Design became the talk of the town with the rise of computers. Why? Because this design produces a lot of data, and computers are the number crunchers that help make sense of it all. So, thanks to our silicon friends, researchers can study complicated questions like, "How do diet AND exercise together affect weight loss?" instead of looking at just one of those factors.

Factorial Design Pros

This design's main selling point is its ability to explore interactions between variables. For instance, maybe a new study drug works really well for young people but not so great for older adults. A factorial design could reveal that age is a crucial factor, something you might miss if you only studied the drug's effectiveness in general. It's like being a detective who looks for clues not just in one room but throughout the entire house.

Factorial Design Cons

However, factorial designs have their own bag of challenges. First off, they can be pretty complicated to set up and run. Imagine coordinating a four-way intersection with lots of cars coming from all directions—you've got to make sure everything runs smoothly, or you'll end up with a traffic jam. Similarly, researchers need to carefully plan how they'll measure and analyze all the different variables.

Factorial Design Uses

Factorial designs are widely used in psychology to untangle the web of factors that influence human behavior. They're also popular in fields like marketing, where companies want to understand how different aspects like price, packaging, and advertising influence a product's success.

And speaking of success, the factorial design has been a hit since statisticians like Ronald A. Fisher (yep, him again!) expanded on it in the early-to-mid 20th century. It offered a more nuanced way of understanding the world, proving that sometimes, to get the full picture, you've got to juggle more than one ball at a time.

So, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback and Quasi-Experimental Design is the versatile player, Factorial Design is the strategist who sees the entire game board and makes moves accordingly.

5) Longitudinal Design

pill bottle

Alright, let's take a step into the world of Longitudinal Design. Picture it as the grand storyteller, the kind who doesn't just tell you about a single event but spins an epic tale that stretches over years or even decades. This design isn't about quick snapshots; it's about capturing the whole movie of someone's life or a long-running process.

You know how you might take a photo every year on your birthday to see how you've changed? Longitudinal Design is kind of like that, but for scientific research.

With Longitudinal Design, instead of measuring something just once, researchers come back again and again, sometimes over many years, to see how things are going. This helps them understand not just what's happening, but why it's happening and how it changes over time.

This design really started to shine in the latter half of the 20th century, when researchers began to realize that some questions can't be answered in a hurry. Think about studies that look at how kids grow up, or research on how a certain medicine affects you over a long period. These aren't things you can rush.

The famous Framingham Heart Study , started in 1948, is a prime example. It's been studying heart health in a small town in Massachusetts for decades, and the findings have shaped what we know about heart disease.

Longitudinal Design Pros

So, what's to love about Longitudinal Design? First off, it's the go-to for studying change over time, whether that's how people age or how a forest recovers from a fire.

Longitudinal Design Cons

But it's not all sunshine and rainbows. Longitudinal studies take a lot of patience and resources. Plus, keeping track of participants over many years can be like herding cats—difficult and full of surprises.

Longitudinal Design Uses

Despite these challenges, longitudinal studies have been key in fields like psychology, sociology, and medicine. They provide the kind of deep, long-term insights that other designs just can't match.

So, if the True Experimental Design is the superstar quarterback, and the Quasi-Experimental Design is the flexible athlete, then the Factorial Design is the strategist, and the Longitudinal Design is the wise elder who has seen it all and has stories to tell.

6) Cross-Sectional Design

Now, let's flip the script and talk about Cross-Sectional Design, the polar opposite of the Longitudinal Design. If Longitudinal is the grand storyteller, think of Cross-Sectional as the snapshot photographer. It captures a single moment in time, like a selfie that you take to remember a fun day. Researchers using this design collect all their data at one point, providing a kind of "snapshot" of whatever they're studying.

In a Cross-Sectional Design, researchers look at multiple groups all at the same time to see how they're different or similar.

This design rose to popularity in the mid-20th century, mainly because it's so quick and efficient. Imagine wanting to know how people of different ages feel about a new video game. Instead of waiting for years to see how opinions change, you could just ask people of all ages what they think right now. That's Cross-Sectional Design for you—fast and straightforward.

You'll find this type of research everywhere from marketing studies to healthcare. For instance, you might have heard about surveys asking people what they think about a new product or political issue. Those are usually cross-sectional studies, aimed at getting a quick read on public opinion.

Cross-Sectional Design Pros

So, what's the big deal with Cross-Sectional Design? Well, it's the go-to when you need answers fast and don't have the time or resources for a more complicated setup.

Cross-Sectional Design Cons

Remember, speed comes with trade-offs. While you get your results quickly, those results are stuck in time. They can't tell you how things change or why they're changing, just what's happening right now.

Cross-Sectional Design Uses

Also, because they're so quick and simple, cross-sectional studies often serve as the first step in research. They give scientists an idea of what's going on so they can decide if it's worth digging deeper. In that way, they're a bit like a movie trailer, giving you a taste of the action to see if you're interested in seeing the whole film.

So, in our lineup of experimental designs, if True Experimental Design is the superstar quarterback and Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, then Cross-Sectional Design is like the speedy running back—fast, agile, but not designed for long, drawn-out plays.

7) Correlational Design

Next on our roster is the Correlational Design, the keen observer of the experimental world. Imagine this design as the person at a party who loves people-watching. They don't interfere or get involved; they just observe and take mental notes about what's going on.

In a correlational study, researchers don't change or control anything; they simply observe and measure how two variables relate to each other.

The correlational design has roots in the early days of psychology and sociology. Pioneers like Sir Francis Galton used it to study how qualities like intelligence or height could be related within families.

This design is all about asking, "Hey, when this thing happens, does that other thing usually happen too?" For example, researchers might study whether students who have more study time get better grades or whether people who exercise more have lower stress levels.

One of the most famous correlational studies you might have heard of is the link between smoking and lung cancer. Back in the mid-20th century, researchers started noticing that people who smoked a lot also seemed to get lung cancer more often. They couldn't say smoking caused cancer—that would require a true experiment—but the strong correlation was a red flag that led to more research and eventually, health warnings.

Correlational Design Pros

This design is great at proving that two (or more) things can be related. Correlational designs can help prove that more detailed research is needed on a topic. They can help us see patterns or possible causes for things that we otherwise might not have realized.

Correlational Design Cons

But here's where you need to be careful: correlational designs can be tricky. Just because two things are related doesn't mean one causes the other. That's like saying, "Every time I wear my lucky socks, my team wins." Well, it's a fun thought, but those socks aren't really controlling the game.

Correlational Design Uses

Despite this limitation, correlational designs are popular in psychology, economics, and epidemiology, to name a few fields. They're often the first step in exploring a possible relationship between variables. Once a strong correlation is found, researchers may decide to conduct more rigorous experimental studies to examine cause and effect.

So, if the True Experimental Design is the superstar quarterback and the Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, the Factorial Design is the strategist, and the Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, then the Correlational Design is the clever scout, identifying interesting patterns but leaving the heavy lifting of proving cause and effect to the other types of designs.

8) Meta-Analysis

Last but not least, let's talk about Meta-Analysis, the librarian of experimental designs.

If other designs are all about creating new research, Meta-Analysis is about gathering up everyone else's research, sorting it, and figuring out what it all means when you put it together.

Imagine a jigsaw puzzle where each piece is a different study. Meta-Analysis is the process of fitting all those pieces together to see the big picture.

The concept of Meta-Analysis started to take shape in the late 20th century, when computers became powerful enough to handle massive amounts of data. It was like someone handed researchers a super-powered magnifying glass, letting them examine multiple studies at the same time to find common trends or results.

You might have heard of the Cochrane Reviews in healthcare . These are big collections of meta-analyses that help doctors and policymakers figure out what treatments work best based on all the research that's been done.

For example, if ten different studies show that a certain medicine helps lower blood pressure, a meta-analysis would pull all that information together to give a more accurate answer.

Meta-Analysis Pros

The beauty of Meta-Analysis is that it can provide really strong evidence. Instead of relying on one study, you're looking at the whole landscape of research on a topic.

Meta-Analysis Cons

However, it does have some downsides. For one, Meta-Analysis is only as good as the studies it includes. If those studies are flawed, the meta-analysis will be too. It's like baking a cake: if you use bad ingredients, it doesn't matter how good your recipe is—the cake won't turn out well.

Meta-Analysis Uses

Despite these challenges, meta-analyses are highly respected and widely used in many fields like medicine, psychology, and education. They help us make sense of a world that's bursting with information by showing us the big picture drawn from many smaller snapshots.

So, in our all-star lineup, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback and Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, the Factorial Design is the strategist, the Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, and the Correlational Design is the scout, then the Meta-Analysis is like the coach, using insights from everyone else's plays to come up with the best game plan.

9) Non-Experimental Design

Now, let's talk about a player who's a bit of an outsider on this team of experimental designs—the Non-Experimental Design. Think of this design as the commentator or the journalist who covers the game but doesn't actually play.

In a Non-Experimental Design, researchers are like reporters gathering facts, but they don't interfere or change anything. They're simply there to describe and analyze.

Non-Experimental Design Pros

So, what's the deal with Non-Experimental Design? Its strength is in description and exploration. It's really good for studying things as they are in the real world, without changing any conditions.

Non-Experimental Design Cons

Because a non-experimental design doesn't manipulate variables, it can't prove cause and effect. It's like a weather reporter: they can tell you it's raining, but they can't tell you why it's raining.

The downside? Since researchers aren't controlling variables, it's hard to rule out other explanations for what they observe. It's like hearing one side of a story—you get an idea of what happened, but it might not be the complete picture.

Non-Experimental Design Uses

Non-Experimental Design has always been a part of research, especially in fields like anthropology, sociology, and some areas of psychology.

For instance, if you've ever heard of studies that describe how people behave in different cultures or what teens like to do in their free time, that's often Non-Experimental Design at work. These studies aim to capture the essence of a situation, like painting a portrait instead of taking a snapshot.

One well-known example you might have heard about is the Kinsey Reports from the 1940s and 1950s, which described sexual behavior in men and women. Researchers interviewed thousands of people but didn't manipulate any variables like you would in a true experiment. They simply collected data to create a comprehensive picture of the subject matter.

So, in our metaphorical team of research designs, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback and Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, Factorial Design is the strategist, Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, Correlational Design is the scout, and Meta-Analysis is the coach, then Non-Experimental Design is the sports journalist—always present, capturing the game, but not part of the action itself.

10) Repeated Measures Design

white rat

Time to meet the Repeated Measures Design, the time traveler of our research team. If this design were a player in a sports game, it would be the one who keeps revisiting past plays to figure out how to improve the next one.

Repeated Measures Design is all about studying the same people or subjects multiple times to see how they change or react under different conditions.

The idea behind Repeated Measures Design isn't new; it's been around since the early days of psychology and medicine. You could say it's a cousin to the Longitudinal Design, but instead of looking at how things naturally change over time, it focuses on how the same group reacts to different things.

Imagine a study looking at how a new energy drink affects people's running speed. Instead of comparing one group that drank the energy drink to another group that didn't, a Repeated Measures Design would have the same group of people run multiple times—once with the energy drink, and once without. This way, you're really zeroing in on the effect of that energy drink, making the results more reliable.

Repeated Measures Design Pros

The strong point of Repeated Measures Design is that it's super focused. Because it uses the same subjects, you don't have to worry about differences between groups messing up your results.

Repeated Measures Design Cons

But the downside? Well, people can get tired or bored if they're tested too many times, which might affect how they respond.

Repeated Measures Design Uses

A famous example of this design is the "Little Albert" experiment, conducted by John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner in 1920. In this study, a young boy was exposed to a white rat and other stimuli several times to see how his emotional responses changed. Though the ethical standards of this experiment are often criticized today, it was groundbreaking in understanding conditioned emotional responses.

In our metaphorical lineup of research designs, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback and Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, Factorial Design is the strategist, Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, Correlational Design is the scout, Meta-Analysis is the coach, and Non-Experimental Design is the journalist, then Repeated Measures Design is the time traveler—always looping back to fine-tune the game plan.

11) Crossover Design

Next up is Crossover Design, the switch-hitter of the research world. If you're familiar with baseball, you'll know a switch-hitter is someone who can bat both right-handed and left-handed.

In a similar way, Crossover Design allows subjects to experience multiple conditions, flipping them around so that everyone gets a turn in each role.

This design is like the utility player on our team—versatile, flexible, and really good at adapting.

The Crossover Design has its roots in medical research and has been popular since the mid-20th century. It's often used in clinical trials to test the effectiveness of different treatments.

Crossover Design Pros

The neat thing about this design is that it allows each participant to serve as their own control group. Imagine you're testing two new kinds of headache medicine. Instead of giving one type to one group and another type to a different group, you'd give both kinds to the same people but at different times.

Crossover Design Cons

What's the big deal with Crossover Design? Its major strength is in reducing the "noise" that comes from individual differences. Since each person experiences all conditions, it's easier to see real effects. However, there's a catch. This design assumes that there's no lasting effect from the first condition when you switch to the second one. That might not always be true. If the first treatment has a long-lasting effect, it could mess up the results when you switch to the second treatment.

Crossover Design Uses

A well-known example of Crossover Design is in studies that look at the effects of different types of diets—like low-carb vs. low-fat diets. Researchers might have participants follow a low-carb diet for a few weeks, then switch them to a low-fat diet. By doing this, they can more accurately measure how each diet affects the same group of people.

In our team of experimental designs, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback and Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, Factorial Design is the strategist, Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, Correlational Design is the scout, Meta-Analysis is the coach, Non-Experimental Design is the journalist, and Repeated Measures Design is the time traveler, then Crossover Design is the versatile utility player—always ready to adapt and play multiple roles to get the most accurate results.

12) Cluster Randomized Design

Meet the Cluster Randomized Design, the team captain of group-focused research. In our imaginary lineup of experimental designs, if other designs focus on individual players, then Cluster Randomized Design is looking at how the entire team functions.

This approach is especially common in educational and community-based research, and it's been gaining traction since the late 20th century.

Here's how Cluster Randomized Design works: Instead of assigning individual people to different conditions, researchers assign entire groups, or "clusters." These could be schools, neighborhoods, or even entire towns. This helps you see how the new method works in a real-world setting.

Imagine you want to see if a new anti-bullying program really works. Instead of selecting individual students, you'd introduce the program to a whole school or maybe even several schools, and then compare the results to schools without the program.

Cluster Randomized Design Pros

Why use Cluster Randomized Design? Well, sometimes it's just not practical to assign conditions at the individual level. For example, you can't really have half a school following a new reading program while the other half sticks with the old one; that would be way too confusing! Cluster Randomization helps get around this problem by treating each "cluster" as its own mini-experiment.

Cluster Randomized Design Cons

There's a downside, too. Because entire groups are assigned to each condition, there's a risk that the groups might be different in some important way that the researchers didn't account for. That's like having one sports team that's full of veterans playing against a team of rookies; the match wouldn't be fair.

Cluster Randomized Design Uses

A famous example is the research conducted to test the effectiveness of different public health interventions, like vaccination programs. Researchers might roll out a vaccination program in one community but not in another, then compare the rates of disease in both.

In our metaphorical research team, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback, Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, Factorial Design is the strategist, Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, Correlational Design is the scout, Meta-Analysis is the coach, Non-Experimental Design is the journalist, Repeated Measures Design is the time traveler, and Crossover Design is the utility player, then Cluster Randomized Design is the team captain—always looking out for the group as a whole.

13) Mixed-Methods Design

Say hello to Mixed-Methods Design, the all-rounder or the "Renaissance player" of our research team.

Mixed-Methods Design uses a blend of both qualitative and quantitative methods to get a more complete picture, just like a Renaissance person who's good at lots of different things. It's like being good at both offense and defense in a sport; you've got all your bases covered!

Mixed-Methods Design is a fairly new kid on the block, becoming more popular in the late 20th and early 21st centuries as researchers began to see the value in using multiple approaches to tackle complex questions. It's the Swiss Army knife in our research toolkit, combining the best parts of other designs to be more versatile.

Here's how it could work: Imagine you're studying the effects of a new educational app on students' math skills. You might use quantitative methods like tests and grades to measure how much the students improve—that's the 'numbers part.'

But you also want to know how the students feel about math now, or why they think they got better or worse. For that, you could conduct interviews or have students fill out journals—that's the 'story part.'

Mixed-Methods Design Pros

So, what's the scoop on Mixed-Methods Design? The strength is its versatility and depth; you're not just getting numbers or stories, you're getting both, which gives a fuller picture.

Mixed-Methods Design Cons

But, it's also more challenging. Imagine trying to play two sports at the same time! You have to be skilled in different research methods and know how to combine them effectively.

Mixed-Methods Design Uses

A high-profile example of Mixed-Methods Design is research on climate change. Scientists use numbers and data to show temperature changes (quantitative), but they also interview people to understand how these changes are affecting communities (qualitative).

In our team of experimental designs, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback, Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, Factorial Design is the strategist, Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, Correlational Design is the scout, Meta-Analysis is the coach, Non-Experimental Design is the journalist, Repeated Measures Design is the time traveler, Crossover Design is the utility player, and Cluster Randomized Design is the team captain, then Mixed-Methods Design is the Renaissance player—skilled in multiple areas and able to bring them all together for a winning strategy.

14) Multivariate Design

Now, let's turn our attention to Multivariate Design, the multitasker of the research world.

If our lineup of research designs were like players on a basketball court, Multivariate Design would be the player dribbling, passing, and shooting all at once. This design doesn't just look at one or two things; it looks at several variables simultaneously to see how they interact and affect each other.

Multivariate Design is like baking a cake with many ingredients. Instead of just looking at how flour affects the cake, you also consider sugar, eggs, and milk all at once. This way, you understand how everything works together to make the cake taste good or bad.

Multivariate Design has been a go-to method in psychology, economics, and social sciences since the latter half of the 20th century. With the advent of computers and advanced statistical software, analyzing multiple variables at once became a lot easier, and Multivariate Design soared in popularity.

Multivariate Design Pros

So, what's the benefit of using Multivariate Design? Its power lies in its complexity. By studying multiple variables at the same time, you can get a really rich, detailed understanding of what's going on.

Multivariate Design Cons

But that complexity can also be a drawback. With so many variables, it can be tough to tell which ones are really making a difference and which ones are just along for the ride.

Multivariate Design Uses

Imagine you're a coach trying to figure out the best strategy to win games. You wouldn't just look at how many points your star player scores; you'd also consider assists, rebounds, turnovers, and maybe even how loud the crowd is. A Multivariate Design would help you understand how all these factors work together to determine whether you win or lose.

A well-known example of Multivariate Design is in market research. Companies often use this approach to figure out how different factors—like price, packaging, and advertising—affect sales. By studying multiple variables at once, they can find the best combination to boost profits.

In our metaphorical research team, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback, Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, Factorial Design is the strategist, Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster, Correlational Design is the scout, Meta-Analysis is the coach, Non-Experimental Design is the journalist, Repeated Measures Design is the time traveler, Crossover Design is the utility player, Cluster Randomized Design is the team captain, and Mixed-Methods Design is the Renaissance player, then Multivariate Design is the multitasker—juggling many variables at once to get a fuller picture of what's happening.

15) Pretest-Posttest Design

Let's introduce Pretest-Posttest Design, the "Before and After" superstar of our research team. You've probably seen those before-and-after pictures in ads for weight loss programs or home renovations, right?

Well, this design is like that, but for science! Pretest-Posttest Design checks out what things are like before the experiment starts and then compares that to what things are like after the experiment ends.

This design is one of the classics, a staple in research for decades across various fields like psychology, education, and healthcare. It's so simple and straightforward that it has stayed popular for a long time.

In Pretest-Posttest Design, you measure your subject's behavior or condition before you introduce any changes—that's your "before" or "pretest." Then you do your experiment, and after it's done, you measure the same thing again—that's your "after" or "posttest."

Pretest-Posttest Design Pros

What makes Pretest-Posttest Design special? It's pretty easy to understand and doesn't require fancy statistics.

Pretest-Posttest Design Cons

But there are some pitfalls. For example, what if the kids in our math example get better at multiplication just because they're older or because they've taken the test before? That would make it hard to tell if the program is really effective or not.

Pretest-Posttest Design Uses

Let's say you're a teacher and you want to know if a new math program helps kids get better at multiplication. First, you'd give all the kids a multiplication test—that's your pretest. Then you'd teach them using the new math program. At the end, you'd give them the same test again—that's your posttest. If the kids do better on the second test, you might conclude that the program works.

One famous use of Pretest-Posttest Design is in evaluating the effectiveness of driver's education courses. Researchers will measure people's driving skills before and after the course to see if they've improved.

16) Solomon Four-Group Design

Next up is the Solomon Four-Group Design, the "chess master" of our research team. This design is all about strategy and careful planning. Named after Richard L. Solomon who introduced it in the 1940s, this method tries to correct some of the weaknesses in simpler designs, like the Pretest-Posttest Design.

Here's how it rolls: The Solomon Four-Group Design uses four different groups to test a hypothesis. Two groups get a pretest, then one of them receives the treatment or intervention, and both get a posttest. The other two groups skip the pretest, and only one of them receives the treatment before they both get a posttest.

Sound complicated? It's like playing 4D chess; you're thinking several moves ahead!

Solomon Four-Group Design Pros

What's the pro and con of the Solomon Four-Group Design? On the plus side, it provides really robust results because it accounts for so many variables.

Solomon Four-Group Design Cons

The downside? It's a lot of work and requires a lot of participants, making it more time-consuming and costly.

Solomon Four-Group Design Uses

Let's say you want to figure out if a new way of teaching history helps students remember facts better. Two classes take a history quiz (pretest), then one class uses the new teaching method while the other sticks with the old way. Both classes take another quiz afterward (posttest).

Meanwhile, two more classes skip the initial quiz, and then one uses the new method before both take the final quiz. Comparing all four groups will give you a much clearer picture of whether the new teaching method works and whether the pretest itself affects the outcome.

The Solomon Four-Group Design is less commonly used than simpler designs but is highly respected for its ability to control for more variables. It's a favorite in educational and psychological research where you really want to dig deep and figure out what's actually causing changes.

17) Adaptive Designs

Now, let's talk about Adaptive Designs, the chameleons of the experimental world.

Imagine you're a detective, and halfway through solving a case, you find a clue that changes everything. You wouldn't just stick to your old plan; you'd adapt and change your approach, right? That's exactly what Adaptive Designs allow researchers to do.

In an Adaptive Design, researchers can make changes to the study as it's happening, based on early results. In a traditional study, once you set your plan, you stick to it from start to finish.

Adaptive Design Pros

This method is particularly useful in fast-paced or high-stakes situations, like developing a new vaccine in the middle of a pandemic. The ability to adapt can save both time and resources, and more importantly, it can save lives by getting effective treatments out faster.

Adaptive Design Cons

But Adaptive Designs aren't without their drawbacks. They can be very complex to plan and carry out, and there's always a risk that the changes made during the study could introduce bias or errors.

Adaptive Design Uses

Adaptive Designs are most often seen in clinical trials, particularly in the medical and pharmaceutical fields.

For instance, if a new drug is showing really promising results, the study might be adjusted to give more participants the new treatment instead of a placebo. Or if one dose level is showing bad side effects, it might be dropped from the study.

The best part is, these changes are pre-planned. Researchers lay out in advance what changes might be made and under what conditions, which helps keep everything scientific and above board.

In terms of applications, besides their heavy usage in medical and pharmaceutical research, Adaptive Designs are also becoming increasingly popular in software testing and market research. In these fields, being able to quickly adjust to early results can give companies a significant advantage.

Adaptive Designs are like the agile startups of the research world—quick to pivot, keen to learn from ongoing results, and focused on rapid, efficient progress. However, they require a great deal of expertise and careful planning to ensure that the adaptability doesn't compromise the integrity of the research.

18) Bayesian Designs

Next, let's dive into Bayesian Designs, the data detectives of the research universe. Named after Thomas Bayes, an 18th-century statistician and minister, this design doesn't just look at what's happening now; it also takes into account what's happened before.

Imagine if you were a detective who not only looked at the evidence in front of you but also used your past cases to make better guesses about your current one. That's the essence of Bayesian Designs.

Bayesian Designs are like detective work in science. As you gather more clues (or data), you update your best guess on what's really happening. This way, your experiment gets smarter as it goes along.

In the world of research, Bayesian Designs are most notably used in areas where you have some prior knowledge that can inform your current study. For example, if earlier research shows that a certain type of medicine usually works well for a specific illness, a Bayesian Design would include that information when studying a new group of patients with the same illness.

Bayesian Design Pros

One of the major advantages of Bayesian Designs is their efficiency. Because they use existing data to inform the current experiment, often fewer resources are needed to reach a reliable conclusion.

Bayesian Design Cons

However, they can be quite complicated to set up and require a deep understanding of both statistics and the subject matter at hand.

Bayesian Design Uses

Bayesian Designs are highly valued in medical research, finance, environmental science, and even in Internet search algorithms. Their ability to continually update and refine hypotheses based on new evidence makes them particularly useful in fields where data is constantly evolving and where quick, informed decisions are crucial.

Here's a real-world example: In the development of personalized medicine, where treatments are tailored to individual patients, Bayesian Designs are invaluable. If a treatment has been effective for patients with similar genetics or symptoms in the past, a Bayesian approach can use that data to predict how well it might work for a new patient.

This type of design is also increasingly popular in machine learning and artificial intelligence. In these fields, Bayesian Designs help algorithms "learn" from past data to make better predictions or decisions in new situations. It's like teaching a computer to be a detective that gets better and better at solving puzzles the more puzzles it sees.

19) Covariate Adaptive Randomization

old person and young person

Now let's turn our attention to Covariate Adaptive Randomization, which you can think of as the "matchmaker" of experimental designs.

Picture a soccer coach trying to create the most balanced teams for a friendly match. They wouldn't just randomly assign players; they'd take into account each player's skills, experience, and other traits.

Covariate Adaptive Randomization is all about creating the most evenly matched groups possible for an experiment.

In traditional randomization, participants are allocated to different groups purely by chance. This is a pretty fair way to do things, but it can sometimes lead to unbalanced groups.

Imagine if all the professional-level players ended up on one soccer team and all the beginners on another; that wouldn't be a very informative match! Covariate Adaptive Randomization fixes this by using important traits or characteristics (called "covariates") to guide the randomization process.

Covariate Adaptive Randomization Pros

The benefits of this design are pretty clear: it aims for balance and fairness, making the final results more trustworthy.

Covariate Adaptive Randomization Cons

But it's not perfect. It can be complex to implement and requires a deep understanding of which characteristics are most important to balance.

Covariate Adaptive Randomization Uses

This design is particularly useful in medical trials. Let's say researchers are testing a new medication for high blood pressure. Participants might have different ages, weights, or pre-existing conditions that could affect the results.

Covariate Adaptive Randomization would make sure that each treatment group has a similar mix of these characteristics, making the results more reliable and easier to interpret.

In practical terms, this design is often seen in clinical trials for new drugs or therapies, but its principles are also applicable in fields like psychology, education, and social sciences.

For instance, in educational research, it might be used to ensure that classrooms being compared have similar distributions of students in terms of academic ability, socioeconomic status, and other factors.

Covariate Adaptive Randomization is like the wise elder of the group, ensuring that everyone has an equal opportunity to show their true capabilities, thereby making the collective results as reliable as possible.

20) Stepped Wedge Design

Let's now focus on the Stepped Wedge Design, a thoughtful and cautious member of the experimental design family.

Imagine you're trying out a new gardening technique, but you're not sure how well it will work. You decide to apply it to one section of your garden first, watch how it performs, and then gradually extend the technique to other sections. This way, you get to see its effects over time and across different conditions. That's basically how Stepped Wedge Design works.

In a Stepped Wedge Design, all participants or clusters start off in the control group, and then, at different times, they 'step' over to the intervention or treatment group. This creates a wedge-like pattern over time where more and more participants receive the treatment as the study progresses. It's like rolling out a new policy in phases, monitoring its impact at each stage before extending it to more people.

Stepped Wedge Design Pros

The Stepped Wedge Design offers several advantages. Firstly, it allows for the study of interventions that are expected to do more good than harm, which makes it ethically appealing.

Secondly, it's useful when resources are limited and it's not feasible to roll out a new treatment to everyone at once. Lastly, because everyone eventually receives the treatment, it can be easier to get buy-in from participants or organizations involved in the study.

Stepped Wedge Design Cons

However, this design can be complex to analyze because it has to account for both the time factor and the changing conditions in each 'step' of the wedge. And like any study where participants know they're receiving an intervention, there's the potential for the results to be influenced by the placebo effect or other biases.

Stepped Wedge Design Uses

This design is particularly useful in health and social care research. For instance, if a hospital wants to implement a new hygiene protocol, it might start in one department, assess its impact, and then roll it out to other departments over time. This allows the hospital to adjust and refine the new protocol based on real-world data before it's fully implemented.

In terms of applications, Stepped Wedge Designs are commonly used in public health initiatives, organizational changes in healthcare settings, and social policy trials. They are particularly useful in situations where an intervention is being rolled out gradually and it's important to understand its impacts at each stage.

21) Sequential Design

Next up is Sequential Design, the dynamic and flexible member of our experimental design family.

Imagine you're playing a video game where you can choose different paths. If you take one path and find a treasure chest, you might decide to continue in that direction. If you hit a dead end, you might backtrack and try a different route. Sequential Design operates in a similar fashion, allowing researchers to make decisions at different stages based on what they've learned so far.

In a Sequential Design, the experiment is broken down into smaller parts, or "sequences." After each sequence, researchers pause to look at the data they've collected. Based on those findings, they then decide whether to stop the experiment because they've got enough information, or to continue and perhaps even modify the next sequence.

Sequential Design Pros

This allows for a more efficient use of resources, as you're only continuing with the experiment if the data suggests it's worth doing so.

One of the great things about Sequential Design is its efficiency. Because you're making data-driven decisions along the way, you can often reach conclusions more quickly and with fewer resources.

Sequential Design Cons

However, it requires careful planning and expertise to ensure that these "stop or go" decisions are made correctly and without bias.

Sequential Design Uses

In terms of its applications, besides healthcare and medicine, Sequential Design is also popular in quality control in manufacturing, environmental monitoring, and financial modeling. In these areas, being able to make quick decisions based on incoming data can be a big advantage.

This design is often used in clinical trials involving new medications or treatments. For example, if early results show that a new drug has significant side effects, the trial can be stopped before more people are exposed to it.

On the flip side, if the drug is showing promising results, the trial might be expanded to include more participants or to extend the testing period.

Think of Sequential Design as the nimble athlete of experimental designs, capable of quick pivots and adjustments to reach the finish line in the most effective way possible. But just like an athlete needs a good coach, this design requires expert oversight to make sure it stays on the right track.

22) Field Experiments

Last but certainly not least, let's explore Field Experiments—the adventurers of the experimental design world.

Picture a scientist leaving the controlled environment of a lab to test a theory in the real world, like a biologist studying animals in their natural habitat or a social scientist observing people in a real community. These are Field Experiments, and they're all about getting out there and gathering data in real-world settings.

Field Experiments embrace the messiness of the real world, unlike laboratory experiments, where everything is controlled down to the smallest detail. This makes them both exciting and challenging.

Field Experiment Pros

On one hand, the results often give us a better understanding of how things work outside the lab.

While Field Experiments offer real-world relevance, they come with challenges like controlling for outside factors and the ethical considerations of intervening in people's lives without their knowledge.

Field Experiment Cons

On the other hand, the lack of control can make it harder to tell exactly what's causing what. Yet, despite these challenges, they remain a valuable tool for researchers who want to understand how theories play out in the real world.

Field Experiment Uses

Let's say a school wants to improve student performance. In a Field Experiment, they might change the school's daily schedule for one semester and keep track of how students perform compared to another school where the schedule remained the same.

Because the study is happening in a real school with real students, the results could be very useful for understanding how the change might work in other schools. But since it's the real world, lots of other factors—like changes in teachers or even the weather—could affect the results.

Field Experiments are widely used in economics, psychology, education, and public policy. For example, you might have heard of the famous "Broken Windows" experiment in the 1980s that looked at how small signs of disorder, like broken windows or graffiti, could encourage more serious crime in neighborhoods. This experiment had a big impact on how cities think about crime prevention.

From the foundational concepts of control groups and independent variables to the sophisticated layouts like Covariate Adaptive Randomization and Sequential Design, it's clear that the realm of experimental design is as varied as it is fascinating.

We've seen that each design has its own special talents, ideal for specific situations. Some designs, like the Classic Controlled Experiment, are like reliable old friends you can always count on.

Others, like Sequential Design, are flexible and adaptable, making quick changes based on what they learn. And let's not forget the adventurous Field Experiments, which take us out of the lab and into the real world to discover things we might not see otherwise.

Choosing the right experimental design is like picking the right tool for the job. The method you choose can make a big difference in how reliable your results are and how much people will trust what you've discovered. And as we've learned, there's a design to suit just about every question, every problem, and every curiosity.

So the next time you read about a new discovery in medicine, psychology, or any other field, you'll have a better understanding of the thought and planning that went into figuring things out. Experimental design is more than just a set of rules; it's a structured way to explore the unknown and answer questions that can change the world.

Related posts:

  • Experimental Psychologist Career (Salary + Duties + Interviews)
  • 40+ Famous Psychologists (Images + Biographies)
  • 11+ Psychology Experiment Ideas (Goals + Methods)
  • The Little Albert Experiment
  • 41+ White Collar Job Examples (Salary + Path)

Reference this article:

About The Author

Photo of author

Free Personality Test

Free Personality Quiz

Free Memory Test

Free Memory Test

Free IQ Test

Free IQ Test

PracticalPie.com is a participant in the Amazon Associates Program. As an Amazon Associate we earn from qualifying purchases.

Follow Us On:

Youtube Facebook Instagram X/Twitter

Psychology Resources

Developmental

Personality

Relationships

Psychologists

Serial Killers

Psychology Tests

Personality Quiz

Memory Test

Depression test

Type A/B Personality Test

© PracticalPsychology. All rights reserved

Privacy Policy | Terms of Use

Enago Academy

Experimental Research Design — 6 mistakes you should never make!

' src=

Since school days’ students perform scientific experiments that provide results that define and prove the laws and theorems in science. These experiments are laid on a strong foundation of experimental research designs.

An experimental research design helps researchers execute their research objectives with more clarity and transparency.

In this article, we will not only discuss the key aspects of experimental research designs but also the issues to avoid and problems to resolve while designing your research study.

Table of Contents

What Is Experimental Research Design?

Experimental research design is a framework of protocols and procedures created to conduct experimental research with a scientific approach using two sets of variables. Herein, the first set of variables acts as a constant, used to measure the differences of the second set. The best example of experimental research methods is quantitative research .

Experimental research helps a researcher gather the necessary data for making better research decisions and determining the facts of a research study.

When Can a Researcher Conduct Experimental Research?

A researcher can conduct experimental research in the following situations —

  • When time is an important factor in establishing a relationship between the cause and effect.
  • When there is an invariable or never-changing behavior between the cause and effect.
  • Finally, when the researcher wishes to understand the importance of the cause and effect.

Importance of Experimental Research Design

To publish significant results, choosing a quality research design forms the foundation to build the research study. Moreover, effective research design helps establish quality decision-making procedures, structures the research to lead to easier data analysis, and addresses the main research question. Therefore, it is essential to cater undivided attention and time to create an experimental research design before beginning the practical experiment.

By creating a research design, a researcher is also giving oneself time to organize the research, set up relevant boundaries for the study, and increase the reliability of the results. Through all these efforts, one could also avoid inconclusive results. If any part of the research design is flawed, it will reflect on the quality of the results derived.

Types of Experimental Research Designs

Based on the methods used to collect data in experimental studies, the experimental research designs are of three primary types:

1. Pre-experimental Research Design

A research study could conduct pre-experimental research design when a group or many groups are under observation after implementing factors of cause and effect of the research. The pre-experimental design will help researchers understand whether further investigation is necessary for the groups under observation.

Pre-experimental research is of three types —

  • One-shot Case Study Research Design
  • One-group Pretest-posttest Research Design
  • Static-group Comparison

2. True Experimental Research Design

A true experimental research design relies on statistical analysis to prove or disprove a researcher’s hypothesis. It is one of the most accurate forms of research because it provides specific scientific evidence. Furthermore, out of all the types of experimental designs, only a true experimental design can establish a cause-effect relationship within a group. However, in a true experiment, a researcher must satisfy these three factors —

  • There is a control group that is not subjected to changes and an experimental group that will experience the changed variables
  • A variable that can be manipulated by the researcher
  • Random distribution of the variables

This type of experimental research is commonly observed in the physical sciences.

3. Quasi-experimental Research Design

The word “Quasi” means similarity. A quasi-experimental design is similar to a true experimental design. However, the difference between the two is the assignment of the control group. In this research design, an independent variable is manipulated, but the participants of a group are not randomly assigned. This type of research design is used in field settings where random assignment is either irrelevant or not required.

The classification of the research subjects, conditions, or groups determines the type of research design to be used.

experimental research design

Advantages of Experimental Research

Experimental research allows you to test your idea in a controlled environment before taking the research to clinical trials. Moreover, it provides the best method to test your theory because of the following advantages:

  • Researchers have firm control over variables to obtain results.
  • The subject does not impact the effectiveness of experimental research. Anyone can implement it for research purposes.
  • The results are specific.
  • Post results analysis, research findings from the same dataset can be repurposed for similar research ideas.
  • Researchers can identify the cause and effect of the hypothesis and further analyze this relationship to determine in-depth ideas.
  • Experimental research makes an ideal starting point. The collected data could be used as a foundation to build new research ideas for further studies.

6 Mistakes to Avoid While Designing Your Research

There is no order to this list, and any one of these issues can seriously compromise the quality of your research. You could refer to the list as a checklist of what to avoid while designing your research.

1. Invalid Theoretical Framework

Usually, researchers miss out on checking if their hypothesis is logical to be tested. If your research design does not have basic assumptions or postulates, then it is fundamentally flawed and you need to rework on your research framework.

2. Inadequate Literature Study

Without a comprehensive research literature review , it is difficult to identify and fill the knowledge and information gaps. Furthermore, you need to clearly state how your research will contribute to the research field, either by adding value to the pertinent literature or challenging previous findings and assumptions.

3. Insufficient or Incorrect Statistical Analysis

Statistical results are one of the most trusted scientific evidence. The ultimate goal of a research experiment is to gain valid and sustainable evidence. Therefore, incorrect statistical analysis could affect the quality of any quantitative research.

4. Undefined Research Problem

This is one of the most basic aspects of research design. The research problem statement must be clear and to do that, you must set the framework for the development of research questions that address the core problems.

5. Research Limitations

Every study has some type of limitations . You should anticipate and incorporate those limitations into your conclusion, as well as the basic research design. Include a statement in your manuscript about any perceived limitations, and how you considered them while designing your experiment and drawing the conclusion.

6. Ethical Implications

The most important yet less talked about topic is the ethical issue. Your research design must include ways to minimize any risk for your participants and also address the research problem or question at hand. If you cannot manage the ethical norms along with your research study, your research objectives and validity could be questioned.

Experimental Research Design Example

In an experimental design, a researcher gathers plant samples and then randomly assigns half the samples to photosynthesize in sunlight and the other half to be kept in a dark box without sunlight, while controlling all the other variables (nutrients, water, soil, etc.)

By comparing their outcomes in biochemical tests, the researcher can confirm that the changes in the plants were due to the sunlight and not the other variables.

Experimental research is often the final form of a study conducted in the research process which is considered to provide conclusive and specific results. But it is not meant for every research. It involves a lot of resources, time, and money and is not easy to conduct, unless a foundation of research is built. Yet it is widely used in research institutes and commercial industries, for its most conclusive results in the scientific approach.

Have you worked on research designs? How was your experience creating an experimental design? What difficulties did you face? Do write to us or comment below and share your insights on experimental research designs!

Frequently Asked Questions

Randomization is important in an experimental research because it ensures unbiased results of the experiment. It also measures the cause-effect relationship on a particular group of interest.

Experimental research design lay the foundation of a research and structures the research to establish quality decision making process.

There are 3 types of experimental research designs. These are pre-experimental research design, true experimental research design, and quasi experimental research design.

The difference between an experimental and a quasi-experimental design are: 1. The assignment of the control group in quasi experimental research is non-random, unlike true experimental design, which is randomly assigned. 2. Experimental research group always has a control group; on the other hand, it may not be always present in quasi experimental research.

Experimental research establishes a cause-effect relationship by testing a theory or hypothesis using experimental groups or control variables. In contrast, descriptive research describes a study or a topic by defining the variables under it and answering the questions related to the same.

' src=

good and valuable

Very very good

Good presentation.

Rate this article Cancel Reply

Your email address will not be published.

example of research experimental

Enago Academy's Most Popular Articles

10 Tips to Prevent Research Papers From Being Retracted

  • Publishing Research

10 Tips to Prevent Research Papers From Being Retracted

Research paper retractions represent a critical event in the scientific community. When a published article…

2024 Scholar Metrics: Unveiling research impact (2019-2023)

  • Industry News

Google Releases 2024 Scholar Metrics, Evaluates Impact of Scholarly Articles

Google has released its 2024 Scholar Metrics, assessing scholarly articles from 2019 to 2023. This…

What is Academic Integrity and How to Uphold it [FREE CHECKLIST]

Ensuring Academic Integrity and Transparency in Academic Research: A comprehensive checklist for researchers

Academic integrity is the foundation upon which the credibility and value of scientific findings are…

7 Step Guide for Optimizing Impactful Research Process

  • Reporting Research

How to Optimize Your Research Process: A step-by-step guide

For researchers across disciplines, the path to uncovering novel findings and insights is often filled…

Launch of "Sony Women in Technology Award with Nature"

  • Trending Now

Breaking Barriers: Sony and Nature unveil “Women in Technology Award”

Sony Group Corporation and the prestigious scientific journal Nature have collaborated to launch the inaugural…

Choosing the Right Analytical Approach: Thematic analysis vs. content analysis for…

Comparing Cross Sectional and Longitudinal Studies: 5 steps for choosing the right…

example of research experimental

Sign-up to read more

Subscribe for free to get unrestricted access to all our resources on research writing and academic publishing including:

  • 2000+ blog articles
  • 50+ Webinars
  • 10+ Expert podcasts
  • 50+ Infographics
  • 10+ Checklists
  • Research Guides

We hate spam too. We promise to protect your privacy and never spam you.

  • AI in Academia
  • Promoting Research
  • Career Corner
  • Diversity and Inclusion
  • Infographics
  • Expert Video Library
  • Other Resources
  • Enago Learn
  • Upcoming & On-Demand Webinars
  • Peer-Review Week 2023
  • Open Access Week 2023
  • Conference Videos
  • Enago Report
  • Journal Finder
  • Enago Plagiarism & AI Grammar Check
  • Editing Services
  • Publication Support Services
  • Research Impact
  • Translation Services
  • Publication solutions
  • AI-Based Solutions
  • Thought Leadership
  • Call for Articles
  • Call for Speakers
  • Author Training
  • Edit Profile

I am looking for Editing/ Proofreading services for my manuscript Tentative date of next journal submission:

example of research experimental

In your opinion, what is the most effective way to improve integrity in the peer review process?

  • Experimental Research Designs: Types, Examples & Methods

busayo.longe

Experimental research is the most familiar type of research design for individuals in the physical sciences and a host of other fields. This is mainly because experimental research is a classical scientific experiment, similar to those performed in high school science classes.

Imagine taking 2 samples of the same plant and exposing one of them to sunlight, while the other is kept away from sunlight. Let the plant exposed to sunlight be called sample A, while the latter is called sample B.

If after the duration of the research, we find out that sample A grows and sample B dies, even though they are both regularly wetted and given the same treatment. Therefore, we can conclude that sunlight will aid growth in all similar plants.

What is Experimental Research?

Experimental research is a scientific approach to research, where one or more independent variables are manipulated and applied to one or more dependent variables to measure their effect on the latter. The effect of the independent variables on the dependent variables is usually observed and recorded over some time, to aid researchers in drawing a reasonable conclusion regarding the relationship between these 2 variable types.

The experimental research method is widely used in physical and social sciences, psychology, and education. It is based on the comparison between two or more groups with a straightforward logic, which may, however, be difficult to execute.

Mostly related to a laboratory test procedure, experimental research designs involve collecting quantitative data and performing statistical analysis on them during research. Therefore, making it an example of quantitative research method .

What are The Types of Experimental Research Design?

The types of experimental research design are determined by the way the researcher assigns subjects to different conditions and groups. They are of 3 types, namely; pre-experimental, quasi-experimental, and true experimental research.

Pre-experimental Research Design

In pre-experimental research design, either a group or various dependent groups are observed for the effect of the application of an independent variable which is presumed to cause change. It is the simplest form of experimental research design and is treated with no control group.

Although very practical, experimental research is lacking in several areas of the true-experimental criteria. The pre-experimental research design is further divided into three types

  • One-shot Case Study Research Design

In this type of experimental study, only one dependent group or variable is considered. The study is carried out after some treatment which was presumed to cause change, making it a posttest study.

  • One-group Pretest-posttest Research Design: 

This research design combines both posttest and pretest study by carrying out a test on a single group before the treatment is administered and after the treatment is administered. With the former being administered at the beginning of treatment and later at the end.

  • Static-group Comparison: 

In a static-group comparison study, 2 or more groups are placed under observation, where only one of the groups is subjected to some treatment while the other groups are held static. All the groups are post-tested, and the observed differences between the groups are assumed to be a result of the treatment.

Quasi-experimental Research Design

  The word “quasi” means partial, half, or pseudo. Therefore, the quasi-experimental research bearing a resemblance to the true experimental research, but not the same.  In quasi-experiments, the participants are not randomly assigned, and as such, they are used in settings where randomization is difficult or impossible.

 This is very common in educational research, where administrators are unwilling to allow the random selection of students for experimental samples.

Some examples of quasi-experimental research design include; the time series, no equivalent control group design, and the counterbalanced design.

True Experimental Research Design

The true experimental research design relies on statistical analysis to approve or disprove a hypothesis. It is the most accurate type of experimental design and may be carried out with or without a pretest on at least 2 randomly assigned dependent subjects.

The true experimental research design must contain a control group, a variable that can be manipulated by the researcher, and the distribution must be random. The classification of true experimental design include:

  • The posttest-only Control Group Design: In this design, subjects are randomly selected and assigned to the 2 groups (control and experimental), and only the experimental group is treated. After close observation, both groups are post-tested, and a conclusion is drawn from the difference between these groups.
  • The pretest-posttest Control Group Design: For this control group design, subjects are randomly assigned to the 2 groups, both are presented, but only the experimental group is treated. After close observation, both groups are post-tested to measure the degree of change in each group.
  • Solomon four-group Design: This is the combination of the pretest-only and the pretest-posttest control groups. In this case, the randomly selected subjects are placed into 4 groups.

The first two of these groups are tested using the posttest-only method, while the other two are tested using the pretest-posttest method.

Examples of Experimental Research

Experimental research examples are different, depending on the type of experimental research design that is being considered. The most basic example of experimental research is laboratory experiments, which may differ in nature depending on the subject of research.

Administering Exams After The End of Semester

During the semester, students in a class are lectured on particular courses and an exam is administered at the end of the semester. In this case, the students are the subjects or dependent variables while the lectures are the independent variables treated on the subjects.

Only one group of carefully selected subjects are considered in this research, making it a pre-experimental research design example. We will also notice that tests are only carried out at the end of the semester, and not at the beginning.

Further making it easy for us to conclude that it is a one-shot case study research. 

Employee Skill Evaluation

Before employing a job seeker, organizations conduct tests that are used to screen out less qualified candidates from the pool of qualified applicants. This way, organizations can determine an employee’s skill set at the point of employment.

In the course of employment, organizations also carry out employee training to improve employee productivity and generally grow the organization. Further evaluation is carried out at the end of each training to test the impact of the training on employee skills, and test for improvement.

Here, the subject is the employee, while the treatment is the training conducted. This is a pretest-posttest control group experimental research example.

Evaluation of Teaching Method

Let us consider an academic institution that wants to evaluate the teaching method of 2 teachers to determine which is best. Imagine a case whereby the students assigned to each teacher is carefully selected probably due to personal request by parents or due to stubbornness and smartness.

This is a no equivalent group design example because the samples are not equal. By evaluating the effectiveness of each teacher’s teaching method this way, we may conclude after a post-test has been carried out.

However, this may be influenced by factors like the natural sweetness of a student. For example, a very smart student will grab more easily than his or her peers irrespective of the method of teaching.

What are the Characteristics of Experimental Research?  

Experimental research contains dependent, independent and extraneous variables. The dependent variables are the variables being treated or manipulated and are sometimes called the subject of the research.

The independent variables are the experimental treatment being exerted on the dependent variables. Extraneous variables, on the other hand, are other factors affecting the experiment that may also contribute to the change.

The setting is where the experiment is carried out. Many experiments are carried out in the laboratory, where control can be exerted on the extraneous variables, thereby eliminating them.

Other experiments are carried out in a less controllable setting. The choice of setting used in research depends on the nature of the experiment being carried out.

  • Multivariable

Experimental research may include multiple independent variables, e.g. time, skills, test scores, etc.

Why Use Experimental Research Design?  

Experimental research design can be majorly used in physical sciences, social sciences, education, and psychology. It is used to make predictions and draw conclusions on a subject matter. 

Some uses of experimental research design are highlighted below.

  • Medicine: Experimental research is used to provide the proper treatment for diseases. In most cases, rather than directly using patients as the research subject, researchers take a sample of the bacteria from the patient’s body and are treated with the developed antibacterial

The changes observed during this period are recorded and evaluated to determine its effectiveness. This process can be carried out using different experimental research methods.

  • Education: Asides from science subjects like Chemistry and Physics which involves teaching students how to perform experimental research, it can also be used in improving the standard of an academic institution. This includes testing students’ knowledge on different topics, coming up with better teaching methods, and the implementation of other programs that will aid student learning.
  • Human Behavior: Social scientists are the ones who mostly use experimental research to test human behaviour. For example, consider 2 people randomly chosen to be the subject of the social interaction research where one person is placed in a room without human interaction for 1 year.

The other person is placed in a room with a few other people, enjoying human interaction. There will be a difference in their behaviour at the end of the experiment.

  • UI/UX: During the product development phase, one of the major aims of the product team is to create a great user experience with the product. Therefore, before launching the final product design, potential are brought in to interact with the product.

For example, when finding it difficult to choose how to position a button or feature on the app interface, a random sample of product testers are allowed to test the 2 samples and how the button positioning influences the user interaction is recorded.

What are the Disadvantages of Experimental Research?  

  • It is highly prone to human error due to its dependency on variable control which may not be properly implemented. These errors could eliminate the validity of the experiment and the research being conducted.
  • Exerting control of extraneous variables may create unrealistic situations. Eliminating real-life variables will result in inaccurate conclusions. This may also result in researchers controlling the variables to suit his or her personal preferences.
  • It is a time-consuming process. So much time is spent on testing dependent variables and waiting for the effect of the manipulation of dependent variables to manifest.
  • It is expensive.
  • It is very risky and may have ethical complications that cannot be ignored. This is common in medical research, where failed trials may lead to a patient’s death or a deteriorating health condition.
  • Experimental research results are not descriptive.
  • Response bias can also be supplied by the subject of the conversation.
  • Human responses in experimental research can be difficult to measure.

What are the Data Collection Methods in Experimental Research?  

Data collection methods in experimental research are the different ways in which data can be collected for experimental research. They are used in different cases, depending on the type of research being carried out.

1. Observational Study

This type of study is carried out over a long period. It measures and observes the variables of interest without changing existing conditions.

When researching the effect of social interaction on human behavior, the subjects who are placed in 2 different environments are observed throughout the research. No matter the kind of absurd behavior that is exhibited by the subject during this period, its condition will not be changed.

This may be a very risky thing to do in medical cases because it may lead to death or worse medical conditions.

2. Simulations

This procedure uses mathematical, physical, or computer models to replicate a real-life process or situation. It is frequently used when the actual situation is too expensive, dangerous, or impractical to replicate in real life.

This method is commonly used in engineering and operational research for learning purposes and sometimes as a tool to estimate possible outcomes of real research. Some common situation software are Simulink, MATLAB, and Simul8.

Not all kinds of experimental research can be carried out using simulation as a data collection tool . It is very impractical for a lot of laboratory-based research that involves chemical processes.

A survey is a tool used to gather relevant data about the characteristics of a population and is one of the most common data collection tools. A survey consists of a group of questions prepared by the researcher, to be answered by the research subject.

Surveys can be shared with the respondents both physically and electronically. When collecting data through surveys, the kind of data collected depends on the respondent, and researchers have limited control over it.

Formplus is the best tool for collecting experimental data using survey s. It has relevant features that will aid the data collection process and can also be used in other aspects of experimental research.

Differences between Experimental and Non-Experimental Research 

1. In experimental research, the researcher can control and manipulate the environment of the research, including the predictor variable which can be changed. On the other hand, non-experimental research cannot be controlled or manipulated by the researcher at will.

This is because it takes place in a real-life setting, where extraneous variables cannot be eliminated. Therefore, it is more difficult to conclude non-experimental studies, even though they are much more flexible and allow for a greater range of study fields.

2. The relationship between cause and effect cannot be established in non-experimental research, while it can be established in experimental research. This may be because many extraneous variables also influence the changes in the research subject, making it difficult to point at a particular variable as the cause of a particular change

3. Independent variables are not introduced, withdrawn, or manipulated in non-experimental designs, but the same may not be said about experimental research.

Experimental Research vs. Alternatives and When to Use Them

1. experimental research vs causal comparative.

Experimental research enables you to control variables and identify how the independent variable affects the dependent variable. Causal-comparative find out the cause-and-effect relationship between the variables by comparing already existing groups that are affected differently by the independent variable.

For example, in an experiment to see how K-12 education affects children and teenager development. An experimental research would split the children into groups, some would get formal K-12 education, while others won’t. This is not ethically right because every child has the right to education. So, what we do instead would be to compare already existing groups of children who are getting formal education with those who due to some circumstances can not.

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Causal-Comparative Research

  • Causal-Comparative:   Strengths:  More realistic than experiments, can be conducted in real-world settings.  Weaknesses:  Establishing causality can be weaker due to the lack of manipulation.

2. Experimental Research vs Correlational Research

When experimenting, you are trying to establish a cause-and-effect relationship between different variables. For example, you are trying to establish the effect of heat on water, the temperature keeps changing (independent variable) and you see how it affects the water (dependent variable).

For correlational research, you are not necessarily interested in the why or the cause-and-effect relationship between the variables, you are focusing on the relationship. Using the same water and temperature example, you are only interested in the fact that they change, you are not investigating which of the variables or other variables causes them to change.

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Correlational Research

3. experimental research vs descriptive research.

With experimental research, you alter the independent variable to see how it affects the dependent variable, but with descriptive research you are simply studying the characteristics of the variable you are studying.

So, in an experiment to see how blown glass reacts to temperature, experimental research would keep altering the temperature to varying levels of high and low to see how it affects the dependent variable (glass). But descriptive research would investigate the glass properties.

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Descriptive Research

4. experimental research vs action research.

Experimental research tests for causal relationships by focusing on one independent variable vs the dependent variable and keeps other variables constant. So, you are testing hypotheses and using the information from the research to contribute to knowledge.

However, with action research, you are using a real-world setting which means you are not controlling variables. You are also performing the research to solve actual problems and improve already established practices.

For example, if you are testing for how long commutes affect workers’ productivity. With experimental research, you would vary the length of commute to see how the time affects work. But with action research, you would account for other factors such as weather, commute route, nutrition, etc. Also, experimental research helps know the relationship between commute time and productivity, while action research helps you look for ways to improve productivity

Pros and Cons of Experimental vs Action Research

Conclusion  .

Experimental research designs are often considered to be the standard in research designs. This is partly due to the common misconception that research is equivalent to scientific experiments—a component of experimental research design.

In this research design, one or more subjects or dependent variables are randomly assigned to different treatments (i.e. independent variables manipulated by the researcher) and the results are observed to conclude. One of the uniqueness of experimental research is in its ability to control the effect of extraneous variables.

Experimental research is suitable for research whose goal is to examine cause-effect relationships, e.g. explanatory research. It can be conducted in the laboratory or field settings, depending on the aim of the research that is being carried out. 

Logo

Connect to Formplus, Get Started Now - It's Free!

  • examples of experimental research
  • experimental research methods
  • types of experimental research
  • busayo.longe

Formplus

You may also like:

Experimental Vs Non-Experimental Research: 15 Key Differences

Differences between experimental and non experimental research on definitions, types, examples, data collection tools, uses, advantages etc.

example of research experimental

Simpson’s Paradox & How to Avoid it in Experimental Research

In this article, we are going to look at Simpson’s Paradox from its historical point and later, we’ll consider its effect in...

What is Experimenter Bias? Definition, Types & Mitigation

In this article, we will look into the concept of experimental bias and how it can be identified in your research

Response vs Explanatory Variables: Definition & Examples

In this article, we’ll be comparing the two types of variables, what they both mean and see some of their real-life applications in research

Formplus - For Seamless Data Collection

Collect data the right way with a versatile data collection tool. try formplus and transform your work productivity today..

Experimental Design: Types, Examples & Methods

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Experimental design refers to how participants are allocated to different groups in an experiment. Types of design include repeated measures, independent groups, and matched pairs designs.

Probably the most common way to design an experiment in psychology is to divide the participants into two groups, the experimental group and the control group, and then introduce a change to the experimental group, not the control group.

The researcher must decide how he/she will allocate their sample to the different experimental groups.  For example, if there are 10 participants, will all 10 participants participate in both groups (e.g., repeated measures), or will the participants be split in half and take part in only one group each?

Three types of experimental designs are commonly used:

1. Independent Measures

Independent measures design, also known as between-groups , is an experimental design where different participants are used in each condition of the independent variable.  This means that each condition of the experiment includes a different group of participants.

This should be done by random allocation, ensuring that each participant has an equal chance of being assigned to one group.

Independent measures involve using two separate groups of participants, one in each condition. For example:

Independent Measures Design 2

  • Con : More people are needed than with the repeated measures design (i.e., more time-consuming).
  • Pro : Avoids order effects (such as practice or fatigue) as people participate in one condition only.  If a person is involved in several conditions, they may become bored, tired, and fed up by the time they come to the second condition or become wise to the requirements of the experiment!
  • Con : Differences between participants in the groups may affect results, for example, variations in age, gender, or social background.  These differences are known as participant variables (i.e., a type of extraneous variable ).
  • Control : After the participants have been recruited, they should be randomly assigned to their groups. This should ensure the groups are similar, on average (reducing participant variables).

2. Repeated Measures Design

Repeated Measures design is an experimental design where the same participants participate in each independent variable condition.  This means that each experiment condition includes the same group of participants.

Repeated Measures design is also known as within-groups or within-subjects design .

  • Pro : As the same participants are used in each condition, participant variables (i.e., individual differences) are reduced.
  • Con : There may be order effects. Order effects refer to the order of the conditions affecting the participants’ behavior.  Performance in the second condition may be better because the participants know what to do (i.e., practice effect).  Or their performance might be worse in the second condition because they are tired (i.e., fatigue effect). This limitation can be controlled using counterbalancing.
  • Pro : Fewer people are needed as they participate in all conditions (i.e., saves time).
  • Control : To combat order effects, the researcher counter-balances the order of the conditions for the participants.  Alternating the order in which participants perform in different conditions of an experiment.

Counterbalancing

Suppose we used a repeated measures design in which all of the participants first learned words in “loud noise” and then learned them in “no noise.”

We expect the participants to learn better in “no noise” because of order effects, such as practice. However, a researcher can control for order effects using counterbalancing.

The sample would be split into two groups: experimental (A) and control (B).  For example, group 1 does ‘A’ then ‘B,’ and group 2 does ‘B’ then ‘A.’ This is to eliminate order effects.

Although order effects occur for each participant, they balance each other out in the results because they occur equally in both groups.

counter balancing

3. Matched Pairs Design

A matched pairs design is an experimental design where pairs of participants are matched in terms of key variables, such as age or socioeconomic status. One member of each pair is then placed into the experimental group and the other member into the control group .

One member of each matched pair must be randomly assigned to the experimental group and the other to the control group.

matched pairs design

  • Con : If one participant drops out, you lose 2 PPs’ data.
  • Pro : Reduces participant variables because the researcher has tried to pair up the participants so that each condition has people with similar abilities and characteristics.
  • Con : Very time-consuming trying to find closely matched pairs.
  • Pro : It avoids order effects, so counterbalancing is not necessary.
  • Con : Impossible to match people exactly unless they are identical twins!
  • Control : Members of each pair should be randomly assigned to conditions. However, this does not solve all these problems.

Experimental design refers to how participants are allocated to an experiment’s different conditions (or IV levels). There are three types:

1. Independent measures / between-groups : Different participants are used in each condition of the independent variable.

2. Repeated measures /within groups : The same participants take part in each condition of the independent variable.

3. Matched pairs : Each condition uses different participants, but they are matched in terms of important characteristics, e.g., gender, age, intelligence, etc.

Learning Check

Read about each of the experiments below. For each experiment, identify (1) which experimental design was used; and (2) why the researcher might have used that design.

1 . To compare the effectiveness of two different types of therapy for depression, depressed patients were assigned to receive either cognitive therapy or behavior therapy for a 12-week period.

The researchers attempted to ensure that the patients in the two groups had similar severity of depressed symptoms by administering a standardized test of depression to each participant, then pairing them according to the severity of their symptoms.

2 . To assess the difference in reading comprehension between 7 and 9-year-olds, a researcher recruited each group from a local primary school. They were given the same passage of text to read and then asked a series of questions to assess their understanding.

3 . To assess the effectiveness of two different ways of teaching reading, a group of 5-year-olds was recruited from a primary school. Their level of reading ability was assessed, and then they were taught using scheme one for 20 weeks.

At the end of this period, their reading was reassessed, and a reading improvement score was calculated. They were then taught using scheme two for a further 20 weeks, and another reading improvement score for this period was calculated. The reading improvement scores for each child were then compared.

4 . To assess the effect of the organization on recall, a researcher randomly assigned student volunteers to two conditions.

Condition one attempted to recall a list of words that were organized into meaningful categories; condition two attempted to recall the same words, randomly grouped on the page.

Experiment Terminology

Ecological validity.

The degree to which an investigation represents real-life experiences.

Experimenter effects

These are the ways that the experimenter can accidentally influence the participant through their appearance or behavior.

Demand characteristics

The clues in an experiment lead the participants to think they know what the researcher is looking for (e.g., the experimenter’s body language).

Independent variable (IV)

The variable the experimenter manipulates (i.e., changes) is assumed to have a direct effect on the dependent variable.

Dependent variable (DV)

Variable the experimenter measures. This is the outcome (i.e., the result) of a study.

Extraneous variables (EV)

All variables which are not independent variables but could affect the results (DV) of the experiment. Extraneous variables should be controlled where possible.

Confounding variables

Variable(s) that have affected the results (DV), apart from the IV. A confounding variable could be an extraneous variable that has not been controlled.

Random Allocation

Randomly allocating participants to independent variable conditions means that all participants should have an equal chance of taking part in each condition.

The principle of random allocation is to avoid bias in how the experiment is carried out and limit the effects of participant variables.

Order effects

Changes in participants’ performance due to their repeating the same or similar test more than once. Examples of order effects include:

(i) practice effect: an improvement in performance on a task due to repetition, for example, because of familiarity with the task;

(ii) fatigue effect: a decrease in performance of a task due to repetition, for example, because of boredom or tiredness.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

  • Privacy Policy

Research Method

Home » Experimental Design – Types, Methods, Guide

Experimental Design – Types, Methods, Guide

Table of Contents

Experimental Research Design

Experimental Design

Experimental design is a process of planning and conducting scientific experiments to investigate a hypothesis or research question. It involves carefully designing an experiment that can test the hypothesis, and controlling for other variables that may influence the results.

Experimental design typically includes identifying the variables that will be manipulated or measured, defining the sample or population to be studied, selecting an appropriate method of sampling, choosing a method for data collection and analysis, and determining the appropriate statistical tests to use.

Types of Experimental Design

Here are the different types of experimental design:

Completely Randomized Design

In this design, participants are randomly assigned to one of two or more groups, and each group is exposed to a different treatment or condition.

Randomized Block Design

This design involves dividing participants into blocks based on a specific characteristic, such as age or gender, and then randomly assigning participants within each block to one of two or more treatment groups.

Factorial Design

In a factorial design, participants are randomly assigned to one of several groups, each of which receives a different combination of two or more independent variables.

Repeated Measures Design

In this design, each participant is exposed to all of the different treatments or conditions, either in a random order or in a predetermined order.

Crossover Design

This design involves randomly assigning participants to one of two or more treatment groups, with each group receiving one treatment during the first phase of the study and then switching to a different treatment during the second phase.

Split-plot Design

In this design, the researcher manipulates one or more variables at different levels and uses a randomized block design to control for other variables.

Nested Design

This design involves grouping participants within larger units, such as schools or households, and then randomly assigning these units to different treatment groups.

Laboratory Experiment

Laboratory experiments are conducted under controlled conditions, which allows for greater precision and accuracy. However, because laboratory conditions are not always representative of real-world conditions, the results of these experiments may not be generalizable to the population at large.

Field Experiment

Field experiments are conducted in naturalistic settings and allow for more realistic observations. However, because field experiments are not as controlled as laboratory experiments, they may be subject to more sources of error.

Experimental Design Methods

Experimental design methods refer to the techniques and procedures used to design and conduct experiments in scientific research. Here are some common experimental design methods:

Randomization

This involves randomly assigning participants to different groups or treatments to ensure that any observed differences between groups are due to the treatment and not to other factors.

Control Group

The use of a control group is an important experimental design method that involves having a group of participants that do not receive the treatment or intervention being studied. The control group is used as a baseline to compare the effects of the treatment group.

Blinding involves keeping participants, researchers, or both unaware of which treatment group participants are in, in order to reduce the risk of bias in the results.

Counterbalancing

This involves systematically varying the order in which participants receive treatments or interventions in order to control for order effects.

Replication

Replication involves conducting the same experiment with different samples or under different conditions to increase the reliability and validity of the results.

This experimental design method involves manipulating multiple independent variables simultaneously to investigate their combined effects on the dependent variable.

This involves dividing participants into subgroups or blocks based on specific characteristics, such as age or gender, in order to reduce the risk of confounding variables.

Data Collection Method

Experimental design data collection methods are techniques and procedures used to collect data in experimental research. Here are some common experimental design data collection methods:

Direct Observation

This method involves observing and recording the behavior or phenomenon of interest in real time. It may involve the use of structured or unstructured observation, and may be conducted in a laboratory or naturalistic setting.

Self-report Measures

Self-report measures involve asking participants to report their thoughts, feelings, or behaviors using questionnaires, surveys, or interviews. These measures may be administered in person or online.

Behavioral Measures

Behavioral measures involve measuring participants’ behavior directly, such as through reaction time tasks or performance tests. These measures may be administered using specialized equipment or software.

Physiological Measures

Physiological measures involve measuring participants’ physiological responses, such as heart rate, blood pressure, or brain activity, using specialized equipment. These measures may be invasive or non-invasive, and may be administered in a laboratory or clinical setting.

Archival Data

Archival data involves using existing records or data, such as medical records, administrative records, or historical documents, as a source of information. These data may be collected from public or private sources.

Computerized Measures

Computerized measures involve using software or computer programs to collect data on participants’ behavior or responses. These measures may include reaction time tasks, cognitive tests, or other types of computer-based assessments.

Video Recording

Video recording involves recording participants’ behavior or interactions using cameras or other recording equipment. This method can be used to capture detailed information about participants’ behavior or to analyze social interactions.

Data Analysis Method

Experimental design data analysis methods refer to the statistical techniques and procedures used to analyze data collected in experimental research. Here are some common experimental design data analysis methods:

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics are used to summarize and describe the data collected in the study. This includes measures such as mean, median, mode, range, and standard deviation.

Inferential Statistics

Inferential statistics are used to make inferences or generalizations about a larger population based on the data collected in the study. This includes hypothesis testing and estimation.

Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)

ANOVA is a statistical technique used to compare means across two or more groups in order to determine whether there are significant differences between the groups. There are several types of ANOVA, including one-way ANOVA, two-way ANOVA, and repeated measures ANOVA.

Regression Analysis

Regression analysis is used to model the relationship between two or more variables in order to determine the strength and direction of the relationship. There are several types of regression analysis, including linear regression, logistic regression, and multiple regression.

Factor Analysis

Factor analysis is used to identify underlying factors or dimensions in a set of variables. This can be used to reduce the complexity of the data and identify patterns in the data.

Structural Equation Modeling (SEM)

SEM is a statistical technique used to model complex relationships between variables. It can be used to test complex theories and models of causality.

Cluster Analysis

Cluster analysis is used to group similar cases or observations together based on similarities or differences in their characteristics.

Time Series Analysis

Time series analysis is used to analyze data collected over time in order to identify trends, patterns, or changes in the data.

Multilevel Modeling

Multilevel modeling is used to analyze data that is nested within multiple levels, such as students nested within schools or employees nested within companies.

Applications of Experimental Design 

Experimental design is a versatile research methodology that can be applied in many fields. Here are some applications of experimental design:

  • Medical Research: Experimental design is commonly used to test new treatments or medications for various medical conditions. This includes clinical trials to evaluate the safety and effectiveness of new drugs or medical devices.
  • Agriculture : Experimental design is used to test new crop varieties, fertilizers, and other agricultural practices. This includes randomized field trials to evaluate the effects of different treatments on crop yield, quality, and pest resistance.
  • Environmental science: Experimental design is used to study the effects of environmental factors, such as pollution or climate change, on ecosystems and wildlife. This includes controlled experiments to study the effects of pollutants on plant growth or animal behavior.
  • Psychology : Experimental design is used to study human behavior and cognitive processes. This includes experiments to test the effects of different interventions, such as therapy or medication, on mental health outcomes.
  • Engineering : Experimental design is used to test new materials, designs, and manufacturing processes in engineering applications. This includes laboratory experiments to test the strength and durability of new materials, or field experiments to test the performance of new technologies.
  • Education : Experimental design is used to evaluate the effectiveness of teaching methods, educational interventions, and programs. This includes randomized controlled trials to compare different teaching methods or evaluate the impact of educational programs on student outcomes.
  • Marketing : Experimental design is used to test the effectiveness of marketing campaigns, pricing strategies, and product designs. This includes experiments to test the impact of different marketing messages or pricing schemes on consumer behavior.

Examples of Experimental Design 

Here are some examples of experimental design in different fields:

  • Example in Medical research : A study that investigates the effectiveness of a new drug treatment for a particular condition. Patients are randomly assigned to either a treatment group or a control group, with the treatment group receiving the new drug and the control group receiving a placebo. The outcomes, such as improvement in symptoms or side effects, are measured and compared between the two groups.
  • Example in Education research: A study that examines the impact of a new teaching method on student learning outcomes. Students are randomly assigned to either a group that receives the new teaching method or a group that receives the traditional teaching method. Student achievement is measured before and after the intervention, and the results are compared between the two groups.
  • Example in Environmental science: A study that tests the effectiveness of a new method for reducing pollution in a river. Two sections of the river are selected, with one section treated with the new method and the other section left untreated. The water quality is measured before and after the intervention, and the results are compared between the two sections.
  • Example in Marketing research: A study that investigates the impact of a new advertising campaign on consumer behavior. Participants are randomly assigned to either a group that is exposed to the new campaign or a group that is not. Their behavior, such as purchasing or product awareness, is measured and compared between the two groups.
  • Example in Social psychology: A study that examines the effect of a new social intervention on reducing prejudice towards a marginalized group. Participants are randomly assigned to either a group that receives the intervention or a control group that does not. Their attitudes and behavior towards the marginalized group are measured before and after the intervention, and the results are compared between the two groups.

When to use Experimental Research Design 

Experimental research design should be used when a researcher wants to establish a cause-and-effect relationship between variables. It is particularly useful when studying the impact of an intervention or treatment on a particular outcome.

Here are some situations where experimental research design may be appropriate:

  • When studying the effects of a new drug or medical treatment: Experimental research design is commonly used in medical research to test the effectiveness and safety of new drugs or medical treatments. By randomly assigning patients to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the treatment is effective in improving health outcomes.
  • When evaluating the effectiveness of an educational intervention: An experimental research design can be used to evaluate the impact of a new teaching method or educational program on student learning outcomes. By randomly assigning students to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the intervention is effective in improving academic performance.
  • When testing the effectiveness of a marketing campaign: An experimental research design can be used to test the effectiveness of different marketing messages or strategies. By randomly assigning participants to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the marketing campaign is effective in changing consumer behavior.
  • When studying the effects of an environmental intervention: Experimental research design can be used to study the impact of environmental interventions, such as pollution reduction programs or conservation efforts. By randomly assigning locations or areas to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the intervention is effective in improving environmental outcomes.
  • When testing the effects of a new technology: An experimental research design can be used to test the effectiveness and safety of new technologies or engineering designs. By randomly assigning participants or locations to treatment and control groups, researchers can determine whether the new technology is effective in achieving its intended purpose.

How to Conduct Experimental Research

Here are the steps to conduct Experimental Research:

  • Identify a Research Question : Start by identifying a research question that you want to answer through the experiment. The question should be clear, specific, and testable.
  • Develop a Hypothesis: Based on your research question, develop a hypothesis that predicts the relationship between the independent and dependent variables. The hypothesis should be clear and testable.
  • Design the Experiment : Determine the type of experimental design you will use, such as a between-subjects design or a within-subjects design. Also, decide on the experimental conditions, such as the number of independent variables, the levels of the independent variable, and the dependent variable to be measured.
  • Select Participants: Select the participants who will take part in the experiment. They should be representative of the population you are interested in studying.
  • Randomly Assign Participants to Groups: If you are using a between-subjects design, randomly assign participants to groups to control for individual differences.
  • Conduct the Experiment : Conduct the experiment by manipulating the independent variable(s) and measuring the dependent variable(s) across the different conditions.
  • Analyze the Data: Analyze the data using appropriate statistical methods to determine if there is a significant effect of the independent variable(s) on the dependent variable(s).
  • Draw Conclusions: Based on the data analysis, draw conclusions about the relationship between the independent and dependent variables. If the results support the hypothesis, then it is accepted. If the results do not support the hypothesis, then it is rejected.
  • Communicate the Results: Finally, communicate the results of the experiment through a research report or presentation. Include the purpose of the study, the methods used, the results obtained, and the conclusions drawn.

Purpose of Experimental Design 

The purpose of experimental design is to control and manipulate one or more independent variables to determine their effect on a dependent variable. Experimental design allows researchers to systematically investigate causal relationships between variables, and to establish cause-and-effect relationships between the independent and dependent variables. Through experimental design, researchers can test hypotheses and make inferences about the population from which the sample was drawn.

Experimental design provides a structured approach to designing and conducting experiments, ensuring that the results are reliable and valid. By carefully controlling for extraneous variables that may affect the outcome of the study, experimental design allows researchers to isolate the effect of the independent variable(s) on the dependent variable(s), and to minimize the influence of other factors that may confound the results.

Experimental design also allows researchers to generalize their findings to the larger population from which the sample was drawn. By randomly selecting participants and using statistical techniques to analyze the data, researchers can make inferences about the larger population with a high degree of confidence.

Overall, the purpose of experimental design is to provide a rigorous, systematic, and scientific method for testing hypotheses and establishing cause-and-effect relationships between variables. Experimental design is a powerful tool for advancing scientific knowledge and informing evidence-based practice in various fields, including psychology, biology, medicine, engineering, and social sciences.

Advantages of Experimental Design 

Experimental design offers several advantages in research. Here are some of the main advantages:

  • Control over extraneous variables: Experimental design allows researchers to control for extraneous variables that may affect the outcome of the study. By manipulating the independent variable and holding all other variables constant, researchers can isolate the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable.
  • Establishing causality: Experimental design allows researchers to establish causality by manipulating the independent variable and observing its effect on the dependent variable. This allows researchers to determine whether changes in the independent variable cause changes in the dependent variable.
  • Replication : Experimental design allows researchers to replicate their experiments to ensure that the findings are consistent and reliable. Replication is important for establishing the validity and generalizability of the findings.
  • Random assignment: Experimental design often involves randomly assigning participants to conditions. This helps to ensure that individual differences between participants are evenly distributed across conditions, which increases the internal validity of the study.
  • Precision : Experimental design allows researchers to measure variables with precision, which can increase the accuracy and reliability of the data.
  • Generalizability : If the study is well-designed, experimental design can increase the generalizability of the findings. By controlling for extraneous variables and using random assignment, researchers can increase the likelihood that the findings will apply to other populations and contexts.

Limitations of Experimental Design

Experimental design has some limitations that researchers should be aware of. Here are some of the main limitations:

  • Artificiality : Experimental design often involves creating artificial situations that may not reflect real-world situations. This can limit the external validity of the findings, or the extent to which the findings can be generalized to real-world settings.
  • Ethical concerns: Some experimental designs may raise ethical concerns, particularly if they involve manipulating variables that could cause harm to participants or if they involve deception.
  • Participant bias : Participants in experimental studies may modify their behavior in response to the experiment, which can lead to participant bias.
  • Limited generalizability: The conditions of the experiment may not reflect the complexities of real-world situations. As a result, the findings may not be applicable to all populations and contexts.
  • Cost and time : Experimental design can be expensive and time-consuming, particularly if the experiment requires specialized equipment or if the sample size is large.
  • Researcher bias : Researchers may unintentionally bias the results of the experiment if they have expectations or preferences for certain outcomes.
  • Lack of feasibility : Experimental design may not be feasible in some cases, particularly if the research question involves variables that cannot be manipulated or controlled.

About the author

' src=

Muhammad Hassan

Researcher, Academic Writer, Web developer

You may also like

Case Study Research

Case Study – Methods, Examples and Guide

Survey Research

Survey Research – Types, Methods, Examples

Observational Research

Observational Research – Methods and Guide

Applied Research

Applied Research – Types, Methods and Examples

Quantitative Research

Quantitative Research – Methods, Types and...

Basic Research

Basic Research – Types, Methods and Examples

  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Skip to footer
  • QuestionPro

survey software icon

  • Solutions Industries Gaming Automotive Sports and events Education Government Travel & Hospitality Financial Services Healthcare Cannabis Technology Use Case AskWhy Communities Audience Contactless surveys Mobile LivePolls Member Experience GDPR Positive People Science 360 Feedback Surveys
  • Resources Blog eBooks Survey Templates Case Studies Training Help center

example of research experimental

Home Market Research

Experimental Research: What it is + Types of designs

Experimental Research Design

Any research conducted under scientifically acceptable conditions uses experimental methods. The success of experimental studies hinges on researchers confirming the change of a variable is based solely on the manipulation of the constant variable. The research should establish a notable cause and effect.

What is Experimental Research?

Experimental research is a study conducted with a scientific approach using two sets of variables. The first set acts as a constant, which you use to measure the differences of the second set. Quantitative research methods , for example, are experimental.

If you don’t have enough data to support your decisions, you must first determine the facts. This research gathers the data necessary to help you make better decisions.

You can conduct experimental research in the following situations:

  • Time is a vital factor in establishing a relationship between cause and effect.
  • Invariable behavior between cause and effect.
  • You wish to understand the importance of cause and effect.

Experimental Research Design Types

The classic experimental design definition is: “The methods used to collect data in experimental studies.”

There are three primary types of experimental design:

  • Pre-experimental research design
  • True experimental research design
  • Quasi-experimental research design

The way you classify research subjects based on conditions or groups determines the type of research design  you should use.

0 1. Pre-Experimental Design

A group, or various groups, are kept under observation after implementing cause and effect factors. You’ll conduct this research to understand whether further investigation is necessary for these particular groups.

You can break down pre-experimental research further into three types:

  • One-shot Case Study Research Design
  • One-group Pretest-posttest Research Design
  • Static-group Comparison

0 2. True Experimental Design

It relies on statistical analysis to prove or disprove a hypothesis, making it the most accurate form of research. Of the types of experimental design, only true design can establish a cause-effect relationship within a group. In a true experiment, three factors need to be satisfied:

  • There is a Control Group, which won’t be subject to changes, and an Experimental Group, which will experience the changed variables.
  • A variable that can be manipulated by the researcher
  • Random distribution

This experimental research method commonly occurs in the physical sciences.

0 3. Quasi-Experimental Design

The word “Quasi” indicates similarity. A quasi-experimental design is similar to an experimental one, but it is not the same. The difference between the two is the assignment of a control group. In this research, an independent variable is manipulated, but the participants of a group are not randomly assigned. Quasi-research is used in field settings where random assignment is either irrelevant or not required.

Importance of Experimental Design

Experimental research is a powerful tool for understanding cause-and-effect relationships. It allows us to manipulate variables and observe the effects, which is crucial for understanding how different factors influence the outcome of a study.

But the importance of experimental research goes beyond that. It’s a critical method for many scientific and academic studies. It allows us to test theories, develop new products, and make groundbreaking discoveries.

For example, this research is essential for developing new drugs and medical treatments. Researchers can understand how a new drug works by manipulating dosage and administration variables and identifying potential side effects.

Similarly, experimental research is used in the field of psychology to test theories and understand human behavior. By manipulating variables such as stimuli, researchers can gain insights into how the brain works and identify new treatment options for mental health disorders.

It is also widely used in the field of education. It allows educators to test new teaching methods and identify what works best. By manipulating variables such as class size, teaching style, and curriculum, researchers can understand how students learn and identify new ways to improve educational outcomes.

In addition, experimental research is a powerful tool for businesses and organizations. By manipulating variables such as marketing strategies, product design, and customer service, companies can understand what works best and identify new opportunities for growth.

Advantages of Experimental Research

When talking about this research, we can think of human life. Babies do their own rudimentary experiments (such as putting objects in their mouths) to learn about the world around them, while older children and teens do experiments at school to learn more about science.

Ancient scientists used this research to prove that their hypotheses were correct. For example, Galileo Galilei and Antoine Lavoisier conducted various experiments to discover key concepts in physics and chemistry. The same is true of modern experts, who use this scientific method to see if new drugs are effective, discover treatments for diseases, and create new electronic devices (among others).

It’s vital to test new ideas or theories. Why put time, effort, and funding into something that may not work?

This research allows you to test your idea in a controlled environment before marketing. It also provides the best method to test your theory thanks to the following advantages:

Advantages of experimental research

  • Researchers have a stronger hold over variables to obtain desired results.
  • The subject or industry does not impact the effectiveness of experimental research. Any industry can implement it for research purposes.
  • The results are specific.
  • After analyzing the results, you can apply your findings to similar ideas or situations.
  • You can identify the cause and effect of a hypothesis. Researchers can further analyze this relationship to determine more in-depth ideas.
  • Experimental research makes an ideal starting point. The data you collect is a foundation for building more ideas and conducting more action research .

Whether you want to know how the public will react to a new product or if a certain food increases the chance of disease, experimental research is the best place to start. Begin your research by finding subjects using  QuestionPro Audience  and other tools today.

LEARN MORE         FREE TRIAL

MORE LIKE THIS

example of research experimental

Customer Experience Lessons from 13,000 Feet — Tuesday CX Thoughts

Aug 20, 2024

insight

Insight: Definition & meaning, types and examples

Aug 19, 2024

employee loyalty

Employee Loyalty: Strategies for Long-Term Business Success 

Jotform vs SurveyMonkey

Jotform vs SurveyMonkey: Which Is Best in 2024

Aug 15, 2024

Other categories

  • Academic Research
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Assessments
  • Brand Awareness
  • Case Studies
  • Communities
  • Consumer Insights
  • Customer effort score
  • Customer Engagement
  • Customer Experience
  • Customer Loyalty
  • Customer Research
  • Customer Satisfaction
  • Employee Benefits
  • Employee Engagement
  • Employee Retention
  • Friday Five
  • General Data Protection Regulation
  • Insights Hub
  • Life@QuestionPro
  • Market Research
  • Mobile diaries
  • Mobile Surveys
  • New Features
  • Online Communities
  • Question Types
  • Questionnaire
  • QuestionPro Products
  • Release Notes
  • Research Tools and Apps
  • Revenue at Risk
  • Survey Templates
  • Training Tips
  • Tuesday CX Thoughts (TCXT)
  • Uncategorized
  • What’s Coming Up
  • Workforce Intelligence

helpful professor logo

15 Experimental Design Examples

15 Experimental Design Examples

Chris Drew (PhD)

Dr. Chris Drew is the founder of the Helpful Professor. He holds a PhD in education and has published over 20 articles in scholarly journals. He is the former editor of the Journal of Learning Development in Higher Education. [Image Descriptor: Photo of Chris]

Learn about our Editorial Process

experimental design types and definition, explained below

Experimental design involves testing an independent variable against a dependent variable. It is a central feature of the scientific method .

A simple example of an experimental design is a clinical trial, where research participants are placed into control and treatment groups in order to determine the degree to which an intervention in the treatment group is effective.

There are three categories of experimental design . They are:

  • Pre-Experimental Design: Testing the effects of the independent variable on a single participant or a small group of participants (e.g. a case study).
  • Quasi-Experimental Design: Testing the effects of the independent variable on a group of participants who aren’t randomly assigned to treatment and control groups (e.g. purposive sampling).
  • True Experimental Design: Testing the effects of the independent variable on a group of participants who are randomly assigned to treatment and control groups in order to infer causality (e.g. clinical trials).

A good research student can look at a design’s methodology and correctly categorize it. Below are some typical examples of experimental designs, with their type indicated.

Experimental Design Examples

The following are examples of experimental design (with their type indicated).

1. Action Research in the Classroom

Type: Pre-Experimental Design

A teacher wants to know if a small group activity will help students learn how to conduct a survey. So, they test the activity out on a few of their classes and make careful observations regarding the outcome.

The teacher might observe that the students respond well to the activity and seem to be learning the material quickly.

However, because there was no comparison group of students that learned how to do a survey with a different methodology, the teacher cannot be certain that the activity is actually the best method for teaching that subject.

2. Study on the Impact of an Advertisement

An advertising firm has assigned two of their best staff to develop a quirky ad about eating a brand’s new breakfast product.

The team puts together an unusual skit that involves characters enjoying the breakfast while engaged in silly gestures and zany background music. The ad agency doesn’t want to spend a great deal of money on the ad just yet, so the commercial is shot with a low budget. The firm then shows the ad to a small group of people just to see their reactions.

Afterwards they determine that the ad had a strong impact on viewers so they move forward with a much larger budget.

3. Case Study

A medical doctor has a hunch that an old treatment regimen might be effective in treating a rare illness.

The treatment has never been used in this manner before. So, the doctor applies the treatment to two of their patients with the illness. After several weeks, the results seem to indicate that the treatment is not causing any change in the illness. The doctor concludes that there is no need to continue the treatment or conduct a larger study with a control condition.

4. Fertilizer and Plant Growth Study

An agricultural farmer is exploring different combinations of nutrients on plant growth, so she does a small experiment.

Instead of spending a lot of time and money applying the different mixes to acres of land and waiting several months to see the results, she decides to apply the fertilizer to some small plants in the lab.

After several weeks, it appears that the plants are responding well. They are growing rapidly and producing dense branching. She shows the plants to her colleagues and they all agree that further testing is needed under better controlled conditions .

5. Mood States Study

A team of psychologists is interested in studying how mood affects altruistic behavior. They are undecided however, on how to put the research participants in a bad mood, so they try a few pilot studies out.

They try one suggestion and make a 3-minute video that shows sad scenes from famous heart-wrenching movies.

They then recruit a few people to watch the clips and measure their mood states afterwards.

The results indicate that people were put in a negative mood, but since there was no control group, the researchers cannot be 100% confident in the clip’s effectiveness.

6. Math Games and Learning Study

Type: Quasi-Experimental Design

Two teachers have developed a set of math games that they think will make learning math more enjoyable for their students. They decide to test out the games on their classes.

So, for two weeks, one teacher has all of her students play the math games. The other teacher uses the standard teaching techniques. At the end of the two weeks, all students take the same math test. The results indicate that students that played the math games did better on the test.

Although the teachers would like to say the games were the cause of the improved performance, they cannot be 100% sure because the study lacked random assignment . There are many other differences between the groups that played the games and those that did not.

Learn More: Random Assignment Examples

7. Economic Impact of Policy

An economic policy institute has decided to test the effectiveness of a new policy on the development of small business. The institute identifies two cities in a third-world country for testing.

The two cities are similar in terms of size, economic output, and other characteristics. The city in which the new policy was implemented showed a much higher growth of small businesses than the other city.

Although the two cities were similar in many ways, the researchers must be cautious in their conclusions. There may exist other differences between the two cities that effected small business growth other than the policy.

8. Parenting Styles and Academic Performance

Psychologists want to understand how parenting style affects children’s academic performance.

So, they identify a large group of parents that have one of four parenting styles: authoritarian, authoritative, permissive, or neglectful. The researchers then compare the grades of each group and discover that children raised with the authoritative parenting style had better grades than the other three groups. Although these results may seem convincing, it turns out that parents that use the authoritative parenting style also have higher SES class and can afford to provide their children with more intellectually enriching activities like summer STEAM camps.

9. Movies and Donations Study

Will the type of movie a person watches affect the likelihood that they donate to a charitable cause? To answer this question, a researcher decides to solicit donations at the exit point of a large theatre.

He chooses to study two types of movies: action-hero and murder mystery. After collecting donations for one month, he tallies the results. Patrons that watched the action-hero movie donated more than those that watched the murder mystery. Can you think of why these results could be due to something other than the movie?

10. Gender and Mindfulness Apps Study

Researchers decide to conduct a study on whether men or women benefit from mindfulness the most. So, they recruit office workers in large corporations at all levels of management.

Then, they divide the research sample up into males and females and ask the participants to use a mindfulness app once each day for at least 15 minutes.

At the end of three weeks, the researchers give all the participants a questionnaire that measures stress and also take swabs from their saliva to measure stress hormones.

The results indicate the women responded much better to the apps than males and showed lower stress levels on both measures.

Unfortunately, it is difficult to conclude that women respond to apps better than men because the researchers could not randomly assign participants to gender. This means that there may be extraneous variables that are causing the results.

11. Eyewitness Testimony Study

Type: True Experimental Design

To study the how leading questions on the memories of eyewitnesses leads to retroactive inference , Loftus and Palmer (1974) conducted a simple experiment consistent with true experimental design.

Research participants all watched the same short video of two cars having an accident. Each were randomly assigned to be asked either one of two versions of a question regarding the accident.

Half of the participants were asked the question “How fast were the two cars going when they smashed into each other?” and the other half were asked “How fast were the two cars going when they contacted each other?”

Participants’ estimates were affected by the wording of the question. Participants that responded to the question with the word “smashed” gave much higher estimates than participants that responded to the word “contacted.”

12. Sports Nutrition Bars Study

A company wants to test the effects of their sports nutrition bars. So, they recruited students on a college campus to participate in their study. The students were randomly assigned to either the treatment condition or control condition.

Participants in the treatment condition ate two nutrition bars. Participants in the control condition ate two similar looking bars that tasted nearly identical, but offered no nutritional value.

One hour after consuming the bars, participants ran on a treadmill at a moderate pace for 15 minutes. The researchers recorded their speed, breathing rates, and level of exhaustion.

The results indicated that participants that ate the nutrition bars ran faster, breathed more easily, and reported feeling less exhausted than participants that ate the non-nutritious bar.

13. Clinical Trials

Medical researchers often use true experiments to assess the effectiveness of various treatment regimens. For a simplified example: people from the population are randomly selected to participate in a study on the effects of a medication on heart disease.

Participants are randomly assigned to either receive the medication or nothing at all. Three months later, all participants are contacted and they are given a full battery of heart disease tests.

The results indicate that participants that received the medication had significantly lower levels of heart disease than participants that received no medication.

14. Leadership Training Study

A large corporation wants to improve the leadership skills of its mid-level managers. The HR department has developed two programs, one online and the other in-person in small classes.

HR randomly selects 120 employees to participate and then randomly assigned them to one of three conditions: one-third are assigned to the online program, one-third to the in-class version, and one-third are put on a waiting list.

The training lasts for 6 weeks and 4 months later, supervisors of the participants are asked to rate their staff in terms of leadership potential. The supervisors were not informed about which of their staff participated in the program.

The results indicated that the in-person participants received the highest ratings from their supervisors. The online class participants came in second, followed by those on the waiting list.

15. Reading Comprehension and Lighting Study

Different wavelengths of light may affect cognitive processing. To put this hypothesis to the test, a researcher randomly assigned students on a college campus to read a history chapter in one of three lighting conditions: natural sunlight, artificial yellow light, and standard fluorescent light.

At the end of the chapter all students took the same exam. The researcher then compared the scores on the exam for students in each condition. The results revealed that natural sunlight produced the best test scores, followed by yellow light and fluorescent light.

Therefore, the researcher concludes that natural sunlight improves reading comprehension.

See Also: Experimental Study vs Observational Study

Experimental design is a central feature of scientific research. When done using true experimental design, causality can be infered, which allows researchers to provide proof that an independent variable affects a dependent variable. This is necessary in just about every field of research, and especially in medical sciences.

Chris

  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 15 Green Flags in a Relationship
  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 15 Signs you're Burnt Out, Not Lazy
  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 15 Toxic Things Parents Say to their Children
  • Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 15 Red Flags Early in a Relationship

Leave a Comment Cancel Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Experimental design: Guide, steps, examples

Last updated

27 April 2023

Reviewed by

Miroslav Damyanov

Short on time? Get an AI generated summary of this article instead

Experimental research design is a scientific framework that allows you to manipulate one or more variables while controlling the test environment. 

When testing a theory or new product, it can be helpful to have a certain level of control and manipulate variables to discover different outcomes. You can use these experiments to determine cause and effect or study variable associations. 

This guide explores the types of experimental design, the steps in designing an experiment, and the advantages and limitations of experimental design. 

Make research less tedious

Dovetail streamlines research to help you uncover and share actionable insights

  • What is experimental research design?

You can determine the relationship between each of the variables by: 

Manipulating one or more independent variables (i.e., stimuli or treatments)

Applying the changes to one or more dependent variables (i.e., test groups or outcomes)

With the ability to analyze the relationship between variables and using measurable data, you can increase the accuracy of the result. 

What is a good experimental design?

A good experimental design requires: 

Significant planning to ensure control over the testing environment

Sound experimental treatments

Properly assigning subjects to treatment groups

Without proper planning, unexpected external variables can alter an experiment's outcome. 

To meet your research goals, your experimental design should include these characteristics:

Provide unbiased estimates of inputs and associated uncertainties

Enable the researcher to detect differences caused by independent variables

Include a plan for analysis and reporting of the results

Provide easily interpretable results with specific conclusions

What's the difference between experimental and quasi-experimental design?

The major difference between experimental and quasi-experimental design is the random assignment of subjects to groups. 

A true experiment relies on certain controls. Typically, the researcher designs the treatment and randomly assigns subjects to control and treatment groups. 

However, these conditions are unethical or impossible to achieve in some situations.

When it's unethical or impractical to assign participants randomly, that’s when a quasi-experimental design comes in. 

This design allows researchers to conduct a similar experiment by assigning subjects to groups based on non-random criteria. 

Another type of quasi-experimental design might occur when the researcher doesn't have control over the treatment but studies pre-existing groups after they receive different treatments.

When can a researcher conduct experimental research?

Various settings and professions can use experimental research to gather information and observe behavior in controlled settings. 

Basically, a researcher can conduct experimental research any time they want to test a theory with variable and dependent controls. 

Experimental research is an option when the project includes an independent variable and a desire to understand the relationship between cause and effect. 

  • The importance of experimental research design

Experimental research enables researchers to conduct studies that provide specific, definitive answers to questions and hypotheses. 

Researchers can test Independent variables in controlled settings to:

Test the effectiveness of a new medication

Design better products for consumers

Answer questions about human health and behavior

Developing a quality research plan means a researcher can accurately answer vital research questions with minimal error. As a result, definitive conclusions can influence the future of the independent variable. 

Types of experimental research designs

There are three main types of experimental research design. The research type you use will depend on the criteria of your experiment, your research budget, and environmental limitations. 

Pre-experimental research design

A pre-experimental research study is a basic observational study that monitors independent variables’ effects. 

During research, you observe one or more groups after applying a treatment to test whether the treatment causes any change. 

The three subtypes of pre-experimental research design are:

One-shot case study research design

This research method introduces a single test group to a single stimulus to study the results at the end of the application. 

After researchers presume the stimulus or treatment has caused changes, they gather results to determine how it affects the test subjects. 

One-group pretest-posttest design

This method uses a single test group but includes a pretest study as a benchmark. The researcher applies a test before and after the group’s exposure to a specific stimulus. 

Static group comparison design

This method includes two or more groups, enabling the researcher to use one group as a control. They apply a stimulus to one group and leave the other group static. 

A posttest study compares the results among groups. 

True experimental research design

A true experiment is the most common research method. It involves statistical analysis to prove or disprove a specific hypothesis . 

Under completely experimental conditions, researchers expose participants in two or more randomized groups to different stimuli. 

Random selection removes any potential for bias, providing more reliable results. 

These are the three main sub-groups of true experimental research design:

Posttest-only control group design

This structure requires the researcher to divide participants into two random groups. One group receives no stimuli and acts as a control while the other group experiences stimuli.

Researchers perform a test at the end of the experiment to observe the stimuli exposure results.

Pretest-posttest control group design

This test also requires two groups. It includes a pretest as a benchmark before introducing the stimulus. 

The pretest introduces multiple ways to test subjects. For instance, if the control group also experiences a change, it reveals that taking the test twice changes the results.

Solomon four-group design

This structure divides subjects into two groups, with two as control groups. Researchers assign the first control group a posttest only and the second control group a pretest and a posttest. 

The two variable groups mirror the control groups, but researchers expose them to stimuli. The ability to differentiate between groups in multiple ways provides researchers with more testing approaches for data-based conclusions. 

Quasi-experimental research design

Although closely related to a true experiment, quasi-experimental research design differs in approach and scope. 

Quasi-experimental research design doesn’t have randomly selected participants. Researchers typically divide the groups in this research by pre-existing differences. 

Quasi-experimental research is more common in educational studies, nursing, or other research projects where it's not ethical or practical to use randomized subject groups.

  • 5 steps for designing an experiment

Experimental research requires a clearly defined plan to outline the research parameters and expected goals. 

Here are five key steps in designing a successful experiment:

Step 1: Define variables and their relationship

Your experiment should begin with a question: What are you hoping to learn through your experiment? 

The relationship between variables in your study will determine your answer.

Define the independent variable (the intended stimuli) and the dependent variable (the expected effect of the stimuli). After identifying these groups, consider how you might control them in your experiment. 

Could natural variations affect your research? If so, your experiment should include a pretest and posttest. 

Step 2: Develop a specific, testable hypothesis

With a firm understanding of the system you intend to study, you can write a specific, testable hypothesis. 

What is the expected outcome of your study? 

Develop a prediction about how the independent variable will affect the dependent variable. 

How will the stimuli in your experiment affect your test subjects? 

Your hypothesis should provide a prediction of the answer to your research question . 

Step 3: Design experimental treatments to manipulate your independent variable

Depending on your experiment, your variable may be a fixed stimulus (like a medical treatment) or a variable stimulus (like a period during which an activity occurs). 

Determine which type of stimulus meets your experiment’s needs and how widely or finely to vary your stimuli. 

Step 4: Assign subjects to groups

When you have a clear idea of how to carry out your experiment, you can determine how to assemble test groups for an accurate study. 

When choosing your study groups, consider: 

The size of your experiment

Whether you can select groups randomly

Your target audience for the outcome of the study

You should be able to create groups with an equal number of subjects and include subjects that match your target audience. Remember, you should assign one group as a control and use one or more groups to study the effects of variables. 

Step 5: Plan how to measure your dependent variable

This step determines how you'll collect data to determine the study's outcome. You should seek reliable and valid measurements that minimize research bias or error. 

You can measure some data with scientific tools, while you’ll need to operationalize other forms to turn them into measurable observations.

  • Advantages of experimental research

Experimental research is an integral part of our world. It allows researchers to conduct experiments that answer specific questions. 

While researchers use many methods to conduct different experiments, experimental research offers these distinct benefits:

Researchers can determine cause and effect by manipulating variables.

It gives researchers a high level of control.

Researchers can test multiple variables within a single experiment.

All industries and fields of knowledge can use it. 

Researchers can duplicate results to promote the validity of the study .

Replicating natural settings rapidly means immediate research.

Researchers can combine it with other research methods.

It provides specific conclusions about the validity of a product, theory, or idea.

  • Disadvantages (or limitations) of experimental research

Unfortunately, no research type yields ideal conditions or perfect results. 

While experimental research might be the right choice for some studies, certain conditions could render experiments useless or even dangerous. 

Before conducting experimental research, consider these disadvantages and limitations:

Required professional qualification

Only competent professionals with an academic degree and specific training are qualified to conduct rigorous experimental research. This ensures results are unbiased and valid. 

Limited scope

Experimental research may not capture the complexity of some phenomena, such as social interactions or cultural norms. These are difficult to control in a laboratory setting.

Resource-intensive

Experimental research can be expensive, time-consuming, and require significant resources, such as specialized equipment or trained personnel.

Limited generalizability

The controlled nature means the research findings may not fully apply to real-world situations or people outside the experimental setting.

Practical or ethical concerns

Some experiments may involve manipulating variables that could harm participants or violate ethical guidelines . 

Researchers must ensure their experiments do not cause harm or discomfort to participants. 

Sometimes, recruiting a sample of people to randomly assign may be difficult. 

  • Experimental research design example

Experiments across all industries and research realms provide scientists, developers, and other researchers with definitive answers. These experiments can solve problems, create inventions, and heal illnesses. 

Product design testing is an excellent example of experimental research. 

A company in the product development phase creates multiple prototypes for testing. With a randomized selection, researchers introduce each test group to a different prototype. 

When groups experience different product designs , the company can assess which option most appeals to potential customers. 

Experimental research design provides researchers with a controlled environment to conduct experiments that evaluate cause and effect. 

Using the five steps to develop a research plan ensures you anticipate and eliminate external variables while answering life’s crucial questions.

Should you be using a customer insights hub?

Do you want to discover previous research faster?

Do you share your research findings with others?

Do you analyze research data?

Start for free today, add your research, and get to key insights faster

Editor’s picks

Last updated: 18 April 2023

Last updated: 27 February 2023

Last updated: 5 February 2023

Last updated: 16 April 2023

Last updated: 16 August 2024

Last updated: 9 March 2023

Last updated: 30 April 2024

Last updated: 12 December 2023

Last updated: 11 March 2024

Last updated: 4 July 2024

Last updated: 6 March 2024

Last updated: 5 March 2024

Last updated: 13 May 2024

Latest articles

Related topics, .css-je19u9{-webkit-align-items:flex-end;-webkit-box-align:flex-end;-ms-flex-align:flex-end;align-items:flex-end;display:-webkit-box;display:-webkit-flex;display:-ms-flexbox;display:flex;-webkit-flex-direction:row;-ms-flex-direction:row;flex-direction:row;-webkit-box-flex-wrap:wrap;-webkit-flex-wrap:wrap;-ms-flex-wrap:wrap;flex-wrap:wrap;-webkit-box-pack:center;-ms-flex-pack:center;-webkit-justify-content:center;justify-content:center;row-gap:0;text-align:center;max-width:671px;}@media (max-width: 1079px){.css-je19u9{max-width:400px;}.css-je19u9>span{white-space:pre;}}@media (max-width: 799px){.css-je19u9{max-width:400px;}.css-je19u9>span{white-space:pre;}} decide what to .css-1kiodld{max-height:56px;display:-webkit-box;display:-webkit-flex;display:-ms-flexbox;display:flex;-webkit-align-items:center;-webkit-box-align:center;-ms-flex-align:center;align-items:center;}@media (max-width: 1079px){.css-1kiodld{display:none;}} build next, decide what to build next.

  • Types of experimental

Log in or sign up

Get started for free

Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, automatically generate references for free.

  • Knowledge Base
  • Methodology

Research Design | Step-by-Step Guide with Examples

Published on 5 May 2022 by Shona McCombes . Revised on 20 March 2023.

A research design is a strategy for answering your research question  using empirical data. Creating a research design means making decisions about:

  • Your overall aims and approach
  • The type of research design you’ll use
  • Your sampling methods or criteria for selecting subjects
  • Your data collection methods
  • The procedures you’ll follow to collect data
  • Your data analysis methods

A well-planned research design helps ensure that your methods match your research aims and that you use the right kind of analysis for your data.

Table of contents

Step 1: consider your aims and approach, step 2: choose a type of research design, step 3: identify your population and sampling method, step 4: choose your data collection methods, step 5: plan your data collection procedures, step 6: decide on your data analysis strategies, frequently asked questions.

  • Introduction

Before you can start designing your research, you should already have a clear idea of the research question you want to investigate.

There are many different ways you could go about answering this question. Your research design choices should be driven by your aims and priorities – start by thinking carefully about what you want to achieve.

The first choice you need to make is whether you’ll take a qualitative or quantitative approach.

Qualitative approach Quantitative approach

Qualitative research designs tend to be more flexible and inductive , allowing you to adjust your approach based on what you find throughout the research process.

Quantitative research designs tend to be more fixed and deductive , with variables and hypotheses clearly defined in advance of data collection.

It’s also possible to use a mixed methods design that integrates aspects of both approaches. By combining qualitative and quantitative insights, you can gain a more complete picture of the problem you’re studying and strengthen the credibility of your conclusions.

Practical and ethical considerations when designing research

As well as scientific considerations, you need to think practically when designing your research. If your research involves people or animals, you also need to consider research ethics .

  • How much time do you have to collect data and write up the research?
  • Will you be able to gain access to the data you need (e.g., by travelling to a specific location or contacting specific people)?
  • Do you have the necessary research skills (e.g., statistical analysis or interview techniques)?
  • Will you need ethical approval ?

At each stage of the research design process, make sure that your choices are practically feasible.

Prevent plagiarism, run a free check.

Within both qualitative and quantitative approaches, there are several types of research design to choose from. Each type provides a framework for the overall shape of your research.

Types of quantitative research designs

Quantitative designs can be split into four main types. Experimental and   quasi-experimental designs allow you to test cause-and-effect relationships, while descriptive and correlational designs allow you to measure variables and describe relationships between them.

Type of design Purpose and characteristics
Experimental
Quasi-experimental
Correlational
Descriptive

With descriptive and correlational designs, you can get a clear picture of characteristics, trends, and relationships as they exist in the real world. However, you can’t draw conclusions about cause and effect (because correlation doesn’t imply causation ).

Experiments are the strongest way to test cause-and-effect relationships without the risk of other variables influencing the results. However, their controlled conditions may not always reflect how things work in the real world. They’re often also more difficult and expensive to implement.

Types of qualitative research designs

Qualitative designs are less strictly defined. This approach is about gaining a rich, detailed understanding of a specific context or phenomenon, and you can often be more creative and flexible in designing your research.

The table below shows some common types of qualitative design. They often have similar approaches in terms of data collection, but focus on different aspects when analysing the data.

Type of design Purpose and characteristics
Grounded theory
Phenomenology

Your research design should clearly define who or what your research will focus on, and how you’ll go about choosing your participants or subjects.

In research, a population is the entire group that you want to draw conclusions about, while a sample is the smaller group of individuals you’ll actually collect data from.

Defining the population

A population can be made up of anything you want to study – plants, animals, organisations, texts, countries, etc. In the social sciences, it most often refers to a group of people.

For example, will you focus on people from a specific demographic, region, or background? Are you interested in people with a certain job or medical condition, or users of a particular product?

The more precisely you define your population, the easier it will be to gather a representative sample.

Sampling methods

Even with a narrowly defined population, it’s rarely possible to collect data from every individual. Instead, you’ll collect data from a sample.

To select a sample, there are two main approaches: probability sampling and non-probability sampling . The sampling method you use affects how confidently you can generalise your results to the population as a whole.

Probability sampling Non-probability sampling

Probability sampling is the most statistically valid option, but it’s often difficult to achieve unless you’re dealing with a very small and accessible population.

For practical reasons, many studies use non-probability sampling, but it’s important to be aware of the limitations and carefully consider potential biases. You should always make an effort to gather a sample that’s as representative as possible of the population.

Case selection in qualitative research

In some types of qualitative designs, sampling may not be relevant.

For example, in an ethnography or a case study, your aim is to deeply understand a specific context, not to generalise to a population. Instead of sampling, you may simply aim to collect as much data as possible about the context you are studying.

In these types of design, you still have to carefully consider your choice of case or community. You should have a clear rationale for why this particular case is suitable for answering your research question.

For example, you might choose a case study that reveals an unusual or neglected aspect of your research problem, or you might choose several very similar or very different cases in order to compare them.

Data collection methods are ways of directly measuring variables and gathering information. They allow you to gain first-hand knowledge and original insights into your research problem.

You can choose just one data collection method, or use several methods in the same study.

Survey methods

Surveys allow you to collect data about opinions, behaviours, experiences, and characteristics by asking people directly. There are two main survey methods to choose from: questionnaires and interviews.

Questionnaires Interviews

Observation methods

Observations allow you to collect data unobtrusively, observing characteristics, behaviours, or social interactions without relying on self-reporting.

Observations may be conducted in real time, taking notes as you observe, or you might make audiovisual recordings for later analysis. They can be qualitative or quantitative.

Quantitative observation

Other methods of data collection

There are many other ways you might collect data depending on your field and topic.

Field Examples of data collection methods
Media & communication Collecting a sample of texts (e.g., speeches, articles, or social media posts) for data on cultural norms and narratives
Psychology Using technologies like neuroimaging, eye-tracking, or computer-based tasks to collect data on things like attention, emotional response, or reaction time
Education Using tests or assignments to collect data on knowledge and skills
Physical sciences Using scientific instruments to collect data on things like weight, blood pressure, or chemical composition

If you’re not sure which methods will work best for your research design, try reading some papers in your field to see what data collection methods they used.

Secondary data

If you don’t have the time or resources to collect data from the population you’re interested in, you can also choose to use secondary data that other researchers already collected – for example, datasets from government surveys or previous studies on your topic.

With this raw data, you can do your own analysis to answer new research questions that weren’t addressed by the original study.

Using secondary data can expand the scope of your research, as you may be able to access much larger and more varied samples than you could collect yourself.

However, it also means you don’t have any control over which variables to measure or how to measure them, so the conclusions you can draw may be limited.

As well as deciding on your methods, you need to plan exactly how you’ll use these methods to collect data that’s consistent, accurate, and unbiased.

Planning systematic procedures is especially important in quantitative research, where you need to precisely define your variables and ensure your measurements are reliable and valid.

Operationalisation

Some variables, like height or age, are easily measured. But often you’ll be dealing with more abstract concepts, like satisfaction, anxiety, or competence. Operationalisation means turning these fuzzy ideas into measurable indicators.

If you’re using observations , which events or actions will you count?

If you’re using surveys , which questions will you ask and what range of responses will be offered?

You may also choose to use or adapt existing materials designed to measure the concept you’re interested in – for example, questionnaires or inventories whose reliability and validity has already been established.

Reliability and validity

Reliability means your results can be consistently reproduced , while validity means that you’re actually measuring the concept you’re interested in.

Reliability Validity

For valid and reliable results, your measurement materials should be thoroughly researched and carefully designed. Plan your procedures to make sure you carry out the same steps in the same way for each participant.

If you’re developing a new questionnaire or other instrument to measure a specific concept, running a pilot study allows you to check its validity and reliability in advance.

Sampling procedures

As well as choosing an appropriate sampling method, you need a concrete plan for how you’ll actually contact and recruit your selected sample.

That means making decisions about things like:

  • How many participants do you need for an adequate sample size?
  • What inclusion and exclusion criteria will you use to identify eligible participants?
  • How will you contact your sample – by mail, online, by phone, or in person?

If you’re using a probability sampling method, it’s important that everyone who is randomly selected actually participates in the study. How will you ensure a high response rate?

If you’re using a non-probability method, how will you avoid bias and ensure a representative sample?

Data management

It’s also important to create a data management plan for organising and storing your data.

Will you need to transcribe interviews or perform data entry for observations? You should anonymise and safeguard any sensitive data, and make sure it’s backed up regularly.

Keeping your data well organised will save time when it comes to analysing them. It can also help other researchers validate and add to your findings.

On their own, raw data can’t answer your research question. The last step of designing your research is planning how you’ll analyse the data.

Quantitative data analysis

In quantitative research, you’ll most likely use some form of statistical analysis . With statistics, you can summarise your sample data, make estimates, and test hypotheses.

Using descriptive statistics , you can summarise your sample data in terms of:

  • The distribution of the data (e.g., the frequency of each score on a test)
  • The central tendency of the data (e.g., the mean to describe the average score)
  • The variability of the data (e.g., the standard deviation to describe how spread out the scores are)

The specific calculations you can do depend on the level of measurement of your variables.

Using inferential statistics , you can:

  • Make estimates about the population based on your sample data.
  • Test hypotheses about a relationship between variables.

Regression and correlation tests look for associations between two or more variables, while comparison tests (such as t tests and ANOVAs ) look for differences in the outcomes of different groups.

Your choice of statistical test depends on various aspects of your research design, including the types of variables you’re dealing with and the distribution of your data.

Qualitative data analysis

In qualitative research, your data will usually be very dense with information and ideas. Instead of summing it up in numbers, you’ll need to comb through the data in detail, interpret its meanings, identify patterns, and extract the parts that are most relevant to your research question.

Two of the most common approaches to doing this are thematic analysis and discourse analysis .

Approach Characteristics
Thematic analysis
Discourse analysis

There are many other ways of analysing qualitative data depending on the aims of your research. To get a sense of potential approaches, try reading some qualitative research papers in your field.

A sample is a subset of individuals from a larger population. Sampling means selecting the group that you will actually collect data from in your research.

For example, if you are researching the opinions of students in your university, you could survey a sample of 100 students.

Statistical sampling allows you to test a hypothesis about the characteristics of a population. There are various sampling methods you can use to ensure that your sample is representative of the population as a whole.

Operationalisation means turning abstract conceptual ideas into measurable observations.

For example, the concept of social anxiety isn’t directly observable, but it can be operationally defined in terms of self-rating scores, behavioural avoidance of crowded places, or physical anxiety symptoms in social situations.

Before collecting data , it’s important to consider how you will operationalise the variables that you want to measure.

The research methods you use depend on the type of data you need to answer your research question .

  • If you want to measure something or test a hypothesis , use quantitative methods . If you want to explore ideas, thoughts, and meanings, use qualitative methods .
  • If you want to analyse a large amount of readily available data, use secondary data. If you want data specific to your purposes with control over how they are generated, collect primary data.
  • If you want to establish cause-and-effect relationships between variables , use experimental methods. If you want to understand the characteristics of a research subject, use descriptive methods.

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the ‘Cite this Scribbr article’ button to automatically add the citation to our free Reference Generator.

McCombes, S. (2023, March 20). Research Design | Step-by-Step Guide with Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved 21 August 2024, from https://www.scribbr.co.uk/research-methods/research-design/

Is this article helpful?

Shona McCombes

Shona McCombes

Experimental Research: Definition, Types, Design, Examples

Appinio Research · 14.05.2024 · 32min read

Experimental Research Definition Types Design Examples

Experimental research is a cornerstone of scientific inquiry, providing a systematic approach to understanding cause-and-effect relationships and advancing knowledge in various fields. At its core, experimental research involves manipulating variables, observing outcomes, and drawing conclusions based on empirical evidence. By controlling factors that could influence the outcome, researchers can isolate the effects of specific variables and make reliable inferences about their impact. This guide offers a step-by-step exploration of experimental research, covering key elements such as research design, data collection, analysis, and ethical considerations. Whether you're a novice researcher seeking to understand the basics or an experienced scientist looking to refine your experimental techniques, this guide will equip you with the knowledge and tools needed to conduct rigorous and insightful research.

What is Experimental Research?

Experimental research is a systematic approach to scientific inquiry that aims to investigate cause-and-effect relationships by manipulating independent variables and observing their effects on dependent variables. Experimental research primarily aims to test hypotheses, make predictions, and draw conclusions based on empirical evidence.

By controlling extraneous variables and randomizing participant assignment, researchers can isolate the effects of specific variables and establish causal relationships. Experimental research is characterized by its rigorous methodology, emphasis on objectivity, and reliance on empirical data to support conclusions.

Importance of Experimental Research

  • Establishing Cause-and-Effect Relationships : Experimental research allows researchers to establish causal relationships between variables by systematically manipulating independent variables and observing their effects on dependent variables. This provides valuable insights into the underlying mechanisms driving phenomena and informs theory development.
  • Testing Hypotheses and Making Predictions : Experimental research provides a structured framework for testing hypotheses and predicting the relationship between variables . By systematically manipulating variables and controlling for confounding factors, researchers can empirically test the validity of their hypotheses and refine theoretical models.
  • Informing Evidence-Based Practice : Experimental research generates empirical evidence that informs evidence-based practice in various fields, including healthcare, education, and business. Experimental research contributes to improving outcomes and informing decision-making in real-world settings by identifying effective interventions, treatments, and strategies.
  • Driving Innovation and Advancement : Experimental research drives innovation and advancement by uncovering new insights, challenging existing assumptions, and pushing the boundaries of knowledge. Through rigorous experimentation and empirical validation, researchers can develop novel solutions to complex problems and contribute to the advancement of science and technology.
  • Enhancing Research Rigor and Validity : Experimental research upholds high research rigor and validity standards by employing systematic methods, controlling for confounding variables, and ensuring replicability of findings. By adhering to rigorous methodology and ethical principles, experimental research produces reliable and credible evidence that withstands scrutiny and contributes to the cumulative body of knowledge.

Experimental research plays a pivotal role in advancing scientific understanding, informing evidence-based practice, and driving innovation across various disciplines. By systematically testing hypotheses, establishing causal relationships, and generating empirical evidence, experimental research contributes to the collective pursuit of knowledge and the improvement of society.

Understanding Experimental Design

Experimental design serves as the blueprint for your study, outlining how you'll manipulate variables and control factors to draw valid conclusions.

Experimental Design Components

Experimental design comprises several essential elements:

  • Independent Variable (IV) : This is the variable manipulated by the researcher. It's what you change to observe its effect on the dependent variable. For example, in a study testing the impact of different study techniques on exam scores, the independent variable might be the study method (e.g., flashcards, reading, or practice quizzes).
  • Dependent Variable (DV) : The dependent variable is what you measure to assess the effect of the independent variable. It's the outcome variable affected by the manipulation of the independent variable. In our study example, the dependent variable would be the exam scores.
  • Control Variables : These factors could influence the outcome but are kept constant or controlled to isolate the effect of the independent variable. Controlling variables helps ensure that any observed changes in the dependent variable can be attributed to manipulating the independent variable rather than other factors.
  • Experimental Group : This group receives the treatment or intervention being tested. It's exposed to the manipulated independent variable. In contrast, the control group does not receive the treatment and serves as a baseline for comparison.

Types of Experimental Designs

Experimental designs can vary based on the research question, the nature of the variables, and the desired level of control. Here are some common types:

  • Between-Subjects Design : In this design, different groups of participants are exposed to varying levels of the independent variable. Each group represents a different experimental condition, and participants are only exposed to one condition. For instance, in a study comparing the effectiveness of two teaching methods, one group of students would use Method A, while another would use Method B.
  • Within-Subjects Design : Also known as repeated measures design , this approach involves exposing the same group of participants to all levels of the independent variable. Participants serve as their own controls, and the order of conditions is typically counterbalanced to control for order effects. For example, participants might be tested on their reaction times under different lighting conditions, with the order of conditions randomized to eliminate any research bias .
  • Mixed Designs : Mixed designs combine elements of both between-subjects and within-subjects designs. This allows researchers to examine both between-group differences and within-group changes over time. Mixed designs help study complex phenomena that involve multiple variables and temporal dynamics.

Factors Influencing Experimental Design Choices

Several factors influence the selection of an appropriate experimental design:

  • Research Question : The nature of your research question will guide your choice of experimental design. Some questions may be better suited to between-subjects designs, while others may require a within-subjects approach.
  • Variables : Consider the number and type of variables involved in your study. A factorial design might be appropriate if you're interested in exploring multiple factors simultaneously. Conversely, if you're focused on investigating the effects of a single variable, a simpler design may suffice.
  • Practical Considerations : Practical constraints such as time, resources, and access to participants can impact your choice of experimental design. Depending on your study's specific requirements, some designs may be more feasible or cost-effective   than others .
  • Ethical Considerations : Ethical concerns, such as the potential risks to participants or the need to minimize harm, should also inform your experimental design choices. Ensure that your design adheres to ethical guidelines and safeguards the rights and well-being of participants.

By carefully considering these factors and selecting an appropriate experimental design, you can ensure that your study is well-designed and capable of yielding meaningful insights.

Experimental Research Elements

When conducting experimental research, understanding the key elements is crucial for designing and executing a robust study. Let's explore each of these elements in detail to ensure your experiment is well-planned and executed effectively.

Independent and Dependent Variables

In experimental research, the independent variable (IV) is the factor that the researcher manipulates or controls, while the dependent variable (DV) is the measured outcome or response. The independent variable is what you change in the experiment to observe its effect on the dependent variable.

For example, in a study investigating the effect of different fertilizers on plant growth, the type of fertilizer used would be the independent variable, while the plant growth (height, number of leaves, etc.) would be the dependent variable.

Control Groups and Experimental Groups

Control groups and experimental groups are essential components of experimental design. The control group serves as a baseline for comparison and does not receive the treatment or intervention being studied. Its purpose is to provide a reference point to assess the effects of the independent variable.

In contrast, the experimental group receives the treatment or intervention and is used to measure the impact of the independent variable. For example, in a drug trial, the control group would receive a placebo, while the experimental group would receive the actual medication.

Randomization and Random Sampling

Randomization is the process of randomly assigning participants to different experimental conditions to minimize biases and ensure that each participant has an equal chance of being assigned to any condition. Randomization helps control for extraneous variables and increases the study's internal validity .

Random sampling, on the other hand, involves selecting a representative sample from the population of interest to generalize the findings to the broader population. Random sampling ensures that each member of the population has an equal chance of being included in the sample, reducing the risk of sampling bias .

Replication and Reliability

Replication involves repeating the experiment to confirm the results and assess the reliability of the findings . It is essential for ensuring the validity of scientific findings and building confidence in the robustness of the results. A study that can be replicated consistently across different settings and by various researchers is considered more reliable. Researchers should strive to design experiments that are easily replicable and transparently report their methods to facilitate replication by others.

Validity: Internal, External, Construct, and Statistical Conclusion Validity

Validity refers to the degree to which an experiment measures what it intends to measure and the extent to which the results can be generalized to other populations or contexts. There are several types of validity that researchers should consider:

  • Internal Validity : Internal validity refers to the extent to which the study accurately assesses the causal relationship between variables. Internal validity is threatened by factors such as confounding variables, selection bias, and experimenter effects. Researchers can enhance internal validity through careful experimental design and control procedures.
  • External Validity : External validity refers to the extent to which the study's findings can be generalized to other populations or settings. External validity is influenced by factors such as the representativeness of the sample and the ecological validity of the experimental conditions. Researchers should consider the relevance and applicability of their findings to real-world situations.
  • Construct Validity : Construct validity refers to the degree to which the study accurately measures the theoretical constructs of interest. Construct validity is concerned with whether the operational definitions of the variables align with the underlying theoretical concepts. Researchers can establish construct validity through careful measurement selection and validation procedures.
  • Statistical Conclusion Validity : Statistical conclusion validity refers to the accuracy of the statistical analyses and conclusions drawn from the data. It ensures that the statistical tests used are appropriate for the data and that the conclusions drawn are warranted. Researchers should use robust statistical methods and report effect sizes and confidence intervals to enhance statistical conclusion validity.

By addressing these elements of experimental research and ensuring the validity and reliability of your study, you can conduct research that contributes meaningfully to the advancement of knowledge in your field.

How to Conduct Experimental Research?

Embarking on an experimental research journey involves a series of well-defined phases, each crucial for the success of your study. Let's explore the pre-experimental, experimental, and post-experimental phases to ensure you're equipped to conduct rigorous and insightful research.

Pre-Experimental Phase

The pre-experimental phase lays the foundation for your study, setting the stage for what's to come. Here's what you need to do:

  • Formulating Research Questions and Hypotheses : Start by clearly defining your research questions and formulating testable hypotheses. Your research questions should be specific, relevant, and aligned with your research objectives. Hypotheses provide a framework for testing the relationships between variables and making predictions about the outcomes of your study.
  • Reviewing Literature and Establishing Theoretical Framework : Dive into existing literature relevant to your research topic and establish a solid theoretical framework. Literature review helps you understand the current state of knowledge, identify research gaps, and build upon existing theories. A well-defined theoretical framework provides a conceptual basis for your study and guides your research design and analysis.

Experimental Phase

The experimental phase is where the magic happens – it's time to put your hypotheses to the test and gather data. Here's what you need to consider:

  • Participant Recruitment and Sampling Techniques : Carefully recruit participants for your study using appropriate sampling techniques . The sample should be representative of the population you're studying to ensure the generalizability of your findings. Consider factors such as sample size , demographics , and inclusion criteria when recruiting participants.
  • Implementing Experimental Procedures : Once you've recruited participants, it's time to implement your experimental procedures. Clearly outline the experimental protocol, including instructions for participants, procedures for administering treatments or interventions, and measures for controlling extraneous variables. Standardize your procedures to ensure consistency across participants and minimize sources of bias.
  • Data Collection and Measurement : Collect data using reliable and valid measurement instruments. Depending on your research questions and variables of interest, data collection methods may include surveys , observations, physiological measurements, or experimental tasks. Ensure that your data collection procedures are ethical, respectful of participants' rights, and designed to minimize errors and biases.

Post-Experimental Phase

In the post-experimental phase, you make sense of your data, draw conclusions, and communicate your findings  to the world . Here's what you need to do:

  • Data Analysis Techniques : Analyze your data using appropriate statistical techniques . Choose methods that are aligned with your research design and hypotheses. Standard statistical analyses include descriptive statistics , inferential statistics (e.g., t-tests , ANOVA ), regression analysis , and correlation analysis. Interpret your findings in the context of your research questions and theoretical framework.
  • Interpreting Results and Drawing Conclusions : Once you've analyzed your data, interpret the results and draw conclusions. Discuss the implications of your findings, including any theoretical, practical, or real-world implications. Consider alternative explanations and limitations of your study and propose avenues for future research. Be transparent about the strengths and weaknesses of your study to enhance the credibility of your conclusions.
  • Reporting Findings : Finally, communicate your findings through research reports, academic papers, or presentations. Follow standard formatting guidelines and adhere to ethical standards for research reporting. Clearly articulate your research objectives, methods, results, and conclusions. Consider your target audience and choose appropriate channels for disseminating your findings to maximize impact and reach.

Chi-Square Calculator :

t-Test Calculator :

One-way ANOVA Calculator :

By meticulously planning and executing each experimental research phase, you can generate valuable insights, advance knowledge in your field, and contribute to scientific progress.

A s you navigate the intricate phases of experimental research, leveraging Appinio can streamline your journey toward actionable insights. With our intuitive platform, you can swiftly gather real-time consumer data, empowering you to make informed decisions with confidence. Say goodbye to the complexities of traditional market research and hello to a seamless, efficient process that puts you in the driver's seat of your research endeavors.

Ready to revolutionize your approach to data-driven decision-making? Book a demo today and discover the power of Appinio in transforming your research experience!

Book a Demo

Experimental Research Examples

Understanding how experimental research is applied in various contexts can provide valuable insights into its practical significance and effectiveness. Here are some examples illustrating the application of experimental research in different domains:

Market Research

Experimental studies are crucial in market research in testing hypotheses, evaluating marketing strategies, and understanding consumer behavior . For example, a company may conduct an experiment to determine the most effective advertising message for a new product. Participants could be exposed to different versions of an advertisement, each emphasizing different product features or appeals.

By measuring variables such as brand recall, purchase intent, and brand perception, researchers can assess the impact of each advertising message and identify the most persuasive approach.

Software as a Service (SaaS)

In the SaaS industry, experimental research is often used to optimize user interfaces, features, and pricing models to enhance user experience and drive engagement. For instance, a SaaS company may conduct A/B tests to compare two versions of its software interface, each with a different layout or navigation structure.

Researchers can identify design elements that lead to higher user satisfaction and retention by tracking user interactions, conversion rates, and customer feedback . Experimental research also enables SaaS companies to test new product features or pricing strategies before full-scale implementation, minimizing risks and maximizing return on investment.

Business Management

Experimental research is increasingly utilized in business management to inform decision-making, improve organizational processes, and drive innovation. For example, a business may conduct an experiment to evaluate the effectiveness of a new training program on employee productivity. Participants could be randomly assigned to either receive the training or serve as a control group.

By measuring performance metrics such as sales revenue, customer satisfaction, and employee turnover, researchers can assess the training program's impact and determine its return on investment. Experimental research in business management provides empirical evidence to support strategic initiatives and optimize resource allocation.

In healthcare , experimental research is instrumental in testing new treatments, interventions, and healthcare delivery models to improve patient outcomes and quality of care. For instance, a clinical trial may be conducted to evaluate the efficacy of a new drug in treating a specific medical condition. Participants are randomly assigned to either receive the experimental drug or a placebo, and their health outcomes are monitored over time.

By comparing the effectiveness of the treatment and placebo groups, researchers can determine the drug's efficacy, safety profile, and potential side effects. Experimental research in healthcare informs evidence-based practice and drives advancements in medical science and patient care.

These examples illustrate the versatility and applicability of experimental research across diverse domains, demonstrating its value in generating actionable insights, informing decision-making, and driving innovation. Whether in market research or healthcare, experimental research provides a rigorous and systematic approach to testing hypotheses, evaluating interventions, and advancing knowledge.

Experimental Research Challenges

Even with careful planning and execution, experimental research can present various challenges. Understanding these challenges and implementing effective solutions is crucial for ensuring the validity and reliability of your study. Here are some common challenges and strategies for addressing them.

Sample Size and Statistical Power

Challenge : Inadequate sample size can limit your study's generalizability and statistical power, making it difficult to detect meaningful effects. Small sample sizes increase the risk of Type II errors (false negatives) and reduce the reliability of your findings.

Solution : Increase your sample size to improve statistical power and enhance the robustness of your results. Conduct a power analysis before starting your study to determine the minimum sample size required to detect the effects of interest with sufficient power. Consider factors such as effect size, alpha level, and desired power when calculating sample size requirements. Additionally, consider using techniques such as bootstrapping or resampling to augment small sample sizes and improve the stability of your estimates.

To enhance the reliability of your experimental research findings, you can leverage our Sample Size Calculator . By determining the optimal sample size based on your desired margin of error, confidence level, and standard deviation, you can ensure the representativeness of your survey results. Don't let inadequate sample sizes hinder the validity of your study and unlock the power of precise research planning!

Confounding Variables and Bias

Challenge : Confounding variables are extraneous factors that co-vary with the independent variable and can distort the relationship between the independent and dependent variables. Confounding variables threaten the internal validity of your study and can lead to erroneous conclusions.

Solution : Implement control measures to minimize the influence of confounding variables on your results. Random assignment of participants to experimental conditions helps distribute confounding variables evenly across groups, reducing their impact on the dependent variable. Additionally, consider using matching or blocking techniques to ensure that groups are comparable on relevant variables. Conduct sensitivity analyses to assess the robustness of your findings to potential confounders and explore alternative explanations for your results.

Researcher Effects and Experimenter Bias

Challenge : Researcher effects and experimenter bias occur when the experimenter's expectations or actions inadvertently influence the study's outcomes. This bias can manifest through subtle cues, unintentional behaviors, or unconscious biases , leading to invalid conclusions.

Solution : Implement double-blind procedures whenever possible to mitigate researcher effects and experimenter bias. Double-blind designs conceal information about the experimental conditions from both the participants and the experimenters, minimizing the potential for bias. Standardize experimental procedures and instructions to ensure consistency across conditions and minimize experimenter variability. Additionally, consider using objective outcome measures or automated data collection procedures to reduce the influence of experimenter bias on subjective assessments.

External Validity and Generalizability

Challenge : External validity refers to the extent to which your study's findings can be generalized to other populations, settings, or conditions. Limited external validity restricts the applicability of your results and may hinder their relevance to real-world contexts.

Solution : Enhance external validity by designing studies closely resembling real-world conditions and populations of interest. Consider using diverse samples  that represent  the target population's demographic, cultural, and ecological variability. Conduct replication studies in different contexts or with different populations to assess the robustness and generalizability of your findings. Additionally, consider conducting meta-analyses or systematic reviews to synthesize evidence from multiple studies and enhance the external validity of your conclusions.

By proactively addressing these challenges and implementing effective solutions, you can strengthen the validity, reliability, and impact of your experimental research. Remember to remain vigilant for potential pitfalls throughout the research process and adapt your strategies as needed to ensure the integrity of your findings.

Advanced Topics in Experimental Research

As you delve deeper into experimental research, you'll encounter advanced topics and methodologies that offer greater complexity and nuance.

Quasi-Experimental Designs

Quasi-experimental designs resemble true experiments but lack random assignment to experimental conditions. They are often used when random assignment is impractical, unethical, or impossible. Quasi-experimental designs allow researchers to investigate cause-and-effect relationships in real-world settings where strict experimental control is challenging. Common examples include:

  • Non-Equivalent Groups Design : This design compares two or more groups that were not created through random assignment. While similar to between-subjects designs, non-equivalent group designs lack the random assignment of participants, increasing the risk of confounding variables.
  • Interrupted Time Series Design : In this design, multiple measurements are taken over time before and after an intervention is introduced. Changes in the dependent variable are assessed over time, allowing researchers to infer the impact of the intervention.
  • Regression Discontinuity Design : This design involves assigning participants to different groups based on a cutoff score on a continuous variable. Participants just above and below the cutoff are treated as if they were randomly assigned to different conditions, allowing researchers to estimate causal effects.

Quasi-experimental designs offer valuable insights into real-world phenomena but require careful consideration of potential confounding variables and limitations inherent to non-random assignment.

Factorial Designs

Factorial designs involve manipulating two or more independent variables simultaneously to examine their main effects and interactions. By systematically varying multiple factors, factorial designs allow researchers to explore complex relationships between variables and identify how they interact to influence outcomes. Common types of factorial designs include:

  • 2x2 Factorial Design : This design manipulates two independent variables, each with two levels. It allows researchers to examine the main effects of each variable as well as any interaction between them.
  • Mixed Factorial Design : In this design, one independent variable is manipulated between subjects, while another is manipulated within subjects. Mixed factorial designs enable researchers to investigate both between-subjects and within-subjects effects simultaneously.

Factorial designs provide a comprehensive understanding of how multiple factors contribute to outcomes and offer greater statistical efficiency compared to studying variables in isolation.

Longitudinal and Cross-Sectional Studies

Longitudinal studies involve collecting data from the same participants over an extended period, allowing researchers to observe changes and trajectories over time. Cross-sectional studies , on the other hand, involve collecting data from different participants at a single point in time, providing a snapshot of the population at that moment. Both longitudinal and cross-sectional studies offer unique advantages and challenges:

  • Longitudinal Studies : Longitudinal designs allow researchers to examine developmental processes, track changes over time, and identify causal relationships. However, longitudinal studies require long-term commitment, are susceptible to attrition and dropout, and may be subject to practice effects and cohort effects.
  • Cross-Sectional Studies : Cross-sectional designs are relatively quick and cost-effective, provide a snapshot of population characteristics, and allow for comparisons across different groups. However, cross-sectional studies cannot assess changes over time or establish causal relationships between variables.

Researchers should carefully consider the research question, objectives, and constraints when choosing between longitudinal and cross-sectional designs.

Meta-Analysis and Systematic Reviews

Meta-analysis and systematic reviews are quantitative methods used to synthesize findings from multiple studies and draw robust conclusions. These methods offer several advantages:

  • Meta-Analysis : Meta-analysis combines the results of multiple studies using statistical techniques to estimate overall effect sizes and assess the consistency of findings across studies. Meta-analysis increases statistical power, enhances generalizability, and provides more precise estimates of effect sizes.
  • Systematic Reviews : Systematic reviews involve systematically searching, appraising, and synthesizing existing literature on a specific topic. Systematic reviews provide a comprehensive summary of the evidence, identify gaps and inconsistencies in the literature, and inform future research directions.

Meta-analysis and systematic reviews are valuable tools for evidence-based practice, guiding policy decisions, and advancing scientific knowledge by aggregating and synthesizing empirical evidence from diverse sources.

By exploring these advanced topics in experimental research, you can expand your methodological toolkit, tackle more complex research questions, and contribute to deeper insights and understanding in your field.

Experimental Research Ethical Considerations

When conducting experimental research, it's imperative to uphold ethical standards and prioritize the well-being and rights of participants. Here are some key ethical considerations to keep in mind throughout the research process:

  • Informed Consent : Obtain informed consent from participants before they participate in your study. Ensure that participants understand the purpose of the study, the procedures involved, any potential risks or benefits, and their right to withdraw from the study at any time without penalty.
  • Protection of Participants' Rights : Respect participants' autonomy, privacy, and confidentiality throughout the research process. Safeguard sensitive information and ensure that participants' identities are protected. Be transparent about how their data will be used and stored.
  • Minimizing Harm and Risks : Take steps to mitigate any potential physical or psychological harm to participants. Conduct a risk assessment before starting your study and implement appropriate measures to reduce risks. Provide support services and resources for participants who may experience distress or adverse effects as a result of their participation.
  • Confidentiality and Data Security : Protect participants' privacy and ensure the security of their data. Use encryption and secure storage methods to prevent unauthorized access to sensitive information. Anonymize data whenever possible to minimize the risk of data breaches or privacy violations.
  • Avoiding Deception : Minimize the use of deception in your research and ensure that any deception is justified by the scientific objectives of the study. If deception is necessary, debrief participants fully at the end of the study and provide them with an opportunity to withdraw their data if they wish.
  • Respecting Diversity and Cultural Sensitivity : Be mindful of participants' diverse backgrounds, cultural norms, and values. Avoid imposing your own cultural biases on participants and ensure that your research is conducted in a culturally sensitive manner. Seek input from diverse stakeholders to ensure your research is inclusive and respectful.
  • Compliance with Ethical Guidelines : Familiarize yourself with relevant ethical guidelines and regulations governing research with human participants, such as those outlined by institutional review boards (IRBs) or ethics committees. Ensure that your research adheres to these guidelines and that any potential ethical concerns are addressed appropriately.
  • Transparency and Openness : Be transparent about your research methods, procedures, and findings. Clearly communicate the purpose of your study, any potential risks or limitations, and how participants' data will be used. Share your research findings openly and responsibly, contributing to the collective body of knowledge in your field.

By prioritizing ethical considerations in your experimental research, you demonstrate integrity, respect, and responsibility as a researcher, fostering trust and credibility in the scientific community.

Conclusion for Experimental Research

Experimental research is a powerful tool for uncovering causal relationships and expanding our understanding of the world around us. By carefully designing experiments, collecting data, and analyzing results, researchers can make meaningful contributions to their fields and address pressing questions. However, conducting experimental research comes with responsibilities. Ethical considerations are paramount to ensure the well-being and rights of participants, as well as the integrity of the research process. Researchers can build trust and credibility in their work by upholding ethical standards and prioritizing participant safety and autonomy. Furthermore, as you continue to explore and innovate in experimental research, you must remain open to new ideas and methodologies. Embracing diversity in perspectives and approaches fosters creativity and innovation, leading to breakthrough discoveries and scientific advancements. By promoting collaboration and sharing findings openly, we can collectively push the boundaries of knowledge and tackle some of society's most pressing challenges.

How to Conduct Research in Minutes?

Discover the power of Appinio , the real-time market research platform revolutionizing experimental research. With Appinio, you can access real-time consumer insights to make better data-driven decisions in minutes. Join the thousands of companies worldwide who trust Appinio to deliver fast, reliable consumer insights.

Here's why you should consider using Appinio for your research needs:

  • From questions to insights in minutes:  With Appinio, you can conduct your own market research and get actionable insights in record time, allowing you to make fast, informed decisions for your business.
  • Intuitive platform for anyone:  You don't need a PhD in research to use Appinio. Our platform is designed to be user-friendly and intuitive so  that anyone  can easily create and launch surveys.
  • Extensive reach and targeting options:  Define your target audience from over 1200 characteristics and survey them in over 90 countries. Our platform ensures you reach the right people for your research needs, no matter where they are.

Register now EN

Get free access to the platform!

Join the loop 💌

Be the first to hear about new updates, product news, and data insights. We'll send it all straight to your inbox.

Get the latest market research news straight to your inbox! 💌

Wait, there's more

What is Employee Experience EX and How to Improve It

20.08.2024 | 30min read

What is Employee Experience (EX) and How to Improve It?

Grow your brand and sales market share with a Mental Availability Brand Health Tracking

19.08.2024 | 14min read

Revolutionizing Brand Health with Mental Availability: Key Takeaways

360-Degree Feedback Survey Process Software Examples

15.08.2024 | 31min read

360-Degree Feedback: Survey, Process, Software, Examples

example of research experimental

Summer is here, and so is the sale. Get a yearly plan with up to 65% off today! 🌴🌞

  • Form Builder
  • Survey Maker
  • AI Form Generator
  • AI Survey Tool
  • AI Quiz Maker
  • Store Builder
  • WordPress Plugin

example of research experimental

HubSpot CRM

example of research experimental

Google Sheets

example of research experimental

Google Analytics

example of research experimental

Microsoft Excel

example of research experimental

  • Popular Forms
  • Job Application Form Template
  • Rental Application Form Template
  • Hotel Accommodation Form Template
  • Online Registration Form Template
  • Employment Application Form Template
  • Application Forms
  • Booking Forms
  • Consent Forms
  • Contact Forms
  • Donation Forms
  • Customer Satisfaction Surveys
  • Employee Satisfaction Surveys
  • Evaluation Surveys
  • Feedback Surveys
  • Market Research Surveys
  • Personality Quiz Template
  • Geography Quiz Template
  • Math Quiz Template
  • Science Quiz Template
  • Vocabulary Quiz Template

Try without registration Quick Start

Read engaging stories, how-to guides, learn about forms.app features.

Inspirational ready-to-use templates for getting started fast and powerful.

Spot-on guides on how to use forms.app and make the most out of it.

example of research experimental

See the technical measures we take and learn how we keep your data safe and secure.

  • Integrations
  • Help Center
  • Sign In Sign Up Free
  • What is experimental research: Definition, types & examples

What is experimental research: Definition, types & examples

Defne Çobanoğlu

Life and its secrets can only be proven right or wrong with experimentation. You can speculate and theorize all you wish, but as William Blake once said, “ The true method of knowledge is experiment. ”

It may be a long process and time-consuming, but it is rewarding like no other. And there are multiple ways and methods of experimentation that can help shed light on matters. In this article, we explained the definition, types of experimental research, and some experimental research examples . Let us get started with the definition!

  • What is experimental research?

Experimental research is the process of carrying out a study conducted with a scientific approach using two or more variables. In other words, it is when you gather two or more variables and compare and test them in controlled environments. 

With experimental research, researchers can also collect detailed information about the participants by doing pre-tests and post-tests to learn even more information about the process. With the result of this type of study, the researcher can make conscious decisions. 

The more control the researcher has over the internal and extraneous variables, the better it is for the results. There may be different circumstances when a balanced experiment is not possible to conduct. That is why are are different research designs to accommodate the needs of researchers.

  • 3 Types of experimental research designs

There is more than one dividing point in experimental research designs that differentiates them from one another. These differences are about whether or not there are pre-tests or post-tests done and how the participants are divided into groups. These differences decide which experimental research design is used.

Types of experimental research designs

Types of experimental research designs

1 - Pre-experimental design

This is the most basic method of experimental study. The researcher doing pre-experimental research evaluates a group of dependent variables after changing the independent variables . The results of this scientific method are not satisfactory, and future studies are planned accordingly. The pre-experimental research can be divided into three types:

A. One shot case study research design

Only one variable is considered in this one-shot case study design. This research method is conducted in the post-test part of a study, and the aim is to observe the changes in the effect of the independent variable.

B. One group pre-test post-test research design

In this type of research, a single group is given a pre-test before a study is conducted and a post-test after the study is conducted. The aim of this one-group pre-test post-test research design is to combine and compare the data collected during these tests. 

C. Static-group comparison

In a static group comparison, 2 or more groups are included in a study where only a group of participants is subjected to a new treatment and the other group of participants is held static. After the study is done, both groups do a post-test evaluation, and the changes are seen as results.

2 - Quasi-experimental design

This research type is quite similar to the experimental design; however, it changes in a few aspects. Quasi-experimental research is done when experimentation is needed for accurate data, but it is not possible to do one because of some limitations. Because you can not deliberately deprive someone of medical treatment or give someone harm, some experiments are ethically impossible. In this experimentation method, the researcher can only manipulate some variables. There are three types of quasi-experimental design:

A. Nonequivalent group designs

A nonequivalent group design is used when participants can not be divided equally and randomly for ethical reasons. Because of this, different variables will be more than one, unlike true experimental research.

B. Regression discontinuity

In this type of research design, the researcher does not divide a group into two to make a study, instead, they make use of a natural threshold or pre-existing dividing point. Only participants below or above the threshold get the treatment, and as the divide is minimal, the difference would be minimal as well.

C. Natural Experiments

In natural experiments, random or irregular assignment of patients makes up control and study groups. And they exist in natural scenarios. Because of this reason, they do not qualify as true experiments as they are based on observation.

3 - True experimental design

In true experimental research, the variables, groups, and settings should be identical to the textbook definition. Grouping of the participant are divided randomly, and controlled variables are chosen carefully. Every aspect of a true experiment should be carefully designed and acted out. And only the results of a true experiment can really be fully accurate . A true experimental design can be divided into 3 parts:

A. Post-test only control group design

In this experimental design, the participants are divided into two groups randomly. They are called experimental and control groups. Only the experimental group gets the treatment, while the other one does not. After the experiment and observation, both groups are given a post-test, and a conclusion is drawn from the results.

B. Pre-test post-test control group

In this method, the participants are divided into two groups once again. Also, only the experimental group gets the treatment. And this time, they are given both pre-tests and post-tests with multiple research methods. Thanks to these multiple tests, the researchers can make sure the changes in the experimental group are directly related to the treatment.

C. Solomon four-group design

This is the most comprehensive method of experimentation. The participants are randomly divided into 4 groups. These four groups include all possible permutations by including both control and non-control groups and post-test or pre-test and post-test control groups. This method enhances the quality of the data.

  • Advantages and disadvantages of experimental research

Just as with any other study, experimental research also has its positive and negative sides. It is up to the researchers to be mindful of these facts before starting their studies. Let us see some advantages and disadvantages of experimental research:

Advantages of experimental research:

  • All the variables are in the researchers’ control, and that means the researcher can influence the experiment according to the research question’s requirements.
  • As you can easily control the variables in the experiment, you can specify the results as much as possible.
  • The results of the study identify a cause-and-effect relation .
  • The results can be as specific as the researcher wants.
  • The result of an experimental design opens the doors for future related studies.

Disadvantages of experimental research:

  • Completing an experiment may take years and even decades, so the results will not be as immediate as some of the other research types.
  • As it involves many steps, participants, and researchers, it may be too expensive for some groups.
  • The possibility of researchers making mistakes and having a bias is high. It is important to stay impartial
  • Human behavior and responses can be difficult to measure unless it is specifically experimental research in psychology.
  • Examples of experimental research

When one does experimental research, that experiment can be about anything. As the variables and environments can be controlled by the researcher, it is possible to have experiments about pretty much any subject. It is especially crucial that it gives critical insight into the cause-and-effect relationships of various elements. Now let us see some important examples of experimental research:

An example of experimental research in science:

When scientists make new medicines or come up with a new type of treatment, they have to test those thoroughly to make sure the results will be unanimous and effective for every individual. In order to make sure of this, they can test the medicine on different people or creatures in different dosages and in different frequencies. They can double-check all the results and have crystal clear results.

An example of experimental research in marketing:

The ideal goal of a marketing product, advertisement, or campaign is to attract attention and create positive emotions in the target audience. Marketers can focus on different elements in different campaigns, change the packaging/outline, and have a different approach. Only then can they be sure about the effectiveness of their approaches. Some methods they can work with are A/B testing, online surveys , or focus groups .

  • Frequently asked questions about experimental research

Is experimental research qualitative or quantitative?

Experimental research can be both qualitative and quantitative according to the nature of the study. Experimental research is quantitative when it provides numerical and provable data. The experiment is qualitative when it provides researchers with participants' experiences, attitudes, or the context in which the experiment is conducted.

What is the difference between quasi-experimental research and experimental research?

In true experimental research, the participants are divided into groups randomly and evenly so as to have an equal distinction. However, in quasi-experimental research, the participants can not be divided equally for ethical or practical reasons. They are chosen non-randomly or by using a pre-existing threshold.

  • Wrapping it up

The experimentation process can be long and time-consuming but highly rewarding as it provides valuable as well as both qualitative and quantitative data. It is a valuable part of research methods and gives insight into the subjects to let people make conscious decisions.

In this article, we have gathered experimental research definition, experimental research types, examples, and pros & cons to work as a guide for your next study. You can also make a successful experiment using pre-test and post-test methods and analyze the findings. For further information on different research types and for all your research information, do not forget to visit our other articles!

Defne is a content writer at forms.app. She is also a translator specializing in literary translation. Defne loves reading, writing, and translating professionally and as a hobby. Her expertise lies in survey research, research methodologies, content writing, and translation.

  • Form Features
  • Data Collection

Table of Contents

Related posts.

A full guide to the Porter’s Value Chain

A full guide to the Porter’s Value Chain

Fatih Serdar Çıtak

What is the difference between sales and marketing?

What is the difference between sales and marketing?

Elif N. Çifçi

What is a free form, and how to create one (in 6 simple steps)

What is a free form, and how to create one (in 6 simple steps)

  • Foundations
  • Write Paper

Search form

  • Experiments
  • Anthropology
  • Self-Esteem
  • Social Anxiety

example of research experimental

Experimental Research

Experimental Research

Experimental research is commonly used in sciences such as sociology and psychology, physics, chemistry, biology and medicine etc.

This article is a part of the guide:

  • Pretest-Posttest
  • Third Variable
  • Research Bias
  • Independent Variable
  • Between Subjects

Browse Full Outline

  • 1 Experimental Research
  • 2.1 Independent Variable
  • 2.2 Dependent Variable
  • 2.3 Controlled Variables
  • 2.4 Third Variable
  • 3.1 Control Group
  • 3.2 Research Bias
  • 3.3.1 Placebo Effect
  • 3.3.2 Double Blind Method
  • 4.1 Randomized Controlled Trials
  • 4.2 Pretest-Posttest
  • 4.3 Solomon Four Group
  • 4.4 Between Subjects
  • 4.5 Within Subject
  • 4.6 Repeated Measures
  • 4.7 Counterbalanced Measures
  • 4.8 Matched Subjects

It is a collection of research designs which use manipulation and controlled testing to understand causal processes. Generally, one or more variables are manipulated to determine their effect on a dependent variable.

The experimental method is a systematic and scientific approach to research in which the researcher manipulates one or more variables, and controls and measures any change in other variables.

Experimental Research is often used where:

  • There is time priority in a causal relationship ( cause precedes effect )
  • There is consistency in a causal relationship (a cause will always lead to the same effect)
  • The magnitude of the correlation is great.

(Reference: en.wikipedia.org)

The word experimental research has a range of definitions. In the strict sense, experimental research is what we call a true experiment .

This is an experiment where the researcher manipulates one variable, and control/randomizes the rest of the variables. It has a control group , the subjects have been randomly assigned between the groups, and the researcher only tests one effect at a time. It is also important to know what variable(s) you want to test and measure.

A very wide definition of experimental research, or a quasi experiment , is research where the scientist actively influences something to observe the consequences. Most experiments tend to fall in between the strict and the wide definition.

A rule of thumb is that physical sciences, such as physics, chemistry and geology tend to define experiments more narrowly than social sciences, such as sociology and psychology, which conduct experiments closer to the wider definition.

example of research experimental

Aims of Experimental Research

Experiments are conducted to be able to predict phenomenons. Typically, an experiment is constructed to be able to explain some kind of causation . Experimental research is important to society - it helps us to improve our everyday lives.

example of research experimental

Identifying the Research Problem

After deciding the topic of interest, the researcher tries to define the research problem . This helps the researcher to focus on a more narrow research area to be able to study it appropriately.  Defining the research problem helps you to formulate a  research hypothesis , which is tested against the  null hypothesis .

The research problem is often operationalizationed , to define how to measure the research problem. The results will depend on the exact measurements that the researcher chooses and may be operationalized differently in another study to test the main conclusions of the study.

An ad hoc analysis is a hypothesis invented after testing is done, to try to explain why the contrary evidence. A poor ad hoc analysis may be seen as the researcher's inability to accept that his/her hypothesis is wrong, while a great ad hoc analysis may lead to more testing and possibly a significant discovery.

Constructing the Experiment

There are various aspects to remember when constructing an experiment. Planning ahead ensures that the experiment is carried out properly and that the results reflect the real world, in the best possible way.

Sampling Groups to Study

Sampling groups correctly is especially important when we have more than one condition in the experiment. One sample group often serves as a control group , whilst others are tested under the experimental conditions.

Deciding the sample groups can be done in using many different sampling techniques. Population sampling may chosen by a number of methods, such as randomization , "quasi-randomization" and pairing.

Reducing sampling errors is vital for getting valid results from experiments. Researchers often adjust the sample size to minimize chances of random errors .

Here are some common sampling techniques :

  • probability sampling
  • non-probability sampling
  • simple random sampling
  • convenience sampling
  • stratified sampling
  • systematic sampling
  • cluster sampling
  • sequential sampling
  • disproportional sampling
  • judgmental sampling
  • snowball sampling
  • quota sampling

Creating the Design

The research design is chosen based on a range of factors. Important factors when choosing the design are feasibility, time, cost, ethics, measurement problems and what you would like to test. The design of the experiment is critical for the validity of the results.

Typical Designs and Features in Experimental Design

  • Pretest-Posttest Design Check whether the groups are different before the manipulation starts and the effect of the manipulation. Pretests sometimes influence the effect.
  • Control Group Control groups are designed to measure research bias and measurement effects, such as the Hawthorne Effect or the Placebo Effect . A control group is a group not receiving the same manipulation as the experimental group. Experiments frequently have 2 conditions, but rarely more than 3 conditions at the same time.
  • Randomized Controlled Trials Randomized Sampling, comparison between an Experimental Group and a Control Group and strict control/randomization of all other variables
  • Solomon Four-Group Design With two control groups and two experimental groups. Half the groups have a pretest and half do not have a pretest. This to test both the effect itself and the effect of the pretest.
  • Between Subjects Design Grouping Participants to Different Conditions
  • Within Subject Design Participants Take Part in the Different Conditions - See also: Repeated Measures Design
  • Counterbalanced Measures Design Testing the effect of the order of treatments when no control group is available/ethical
  • Matched Subjects Design Matching Participants to Create Similar Experimental- and Control-Groups
  • Double-Blind Experiment Neither the researcher, nor the participants, know which is the control group. The results can be affected if the researcher or participants know this.
  • Bayesian Probability Using bayesian probability to "interact" with participants is a more "advanced" experimental design. It can be used for settings were there are many variables which are hard to isolate. The researcher starts with a set of initial beliefs, and tries to adjust them to how participants have responded

Pilot Study

It may be wise to first conduct a pilot-study or two before you do the real experiment. This ensures that the experiment measures what it should, and that everything is set up right.

Minor errors, which could potentially destroy the experiment, are often found during this process. With a pilot study, you can get information about errors and problems, and improve the design, before putting a lot of effort into the real experiment.

If the experiments involve humans, a common strategy is to first have a pilot study with someone involved in the research, but not too closely, and then arrange a pilot with a person who resembles the subject(s) . Those two different pilots are likely to give the researcher good information about any problems in the experiment.

Conducting the Experiment

An experiment is typically carried out by manipulating a variable, called the independent variable , affecting the experimental group. The effect that the researcher is interested in, the dependent variable(s) , is measured.

Identifying and controlling non-experimental factors which the researcher does not want to influence the effects, is crucial to drawing a valid conclusion. This is often done by controlling variables , if possible, or randomizing variables to minimize effects that can be traced back to third variables . Researchers only want to measure the effect of the independent variable(s) when conducting an experiment , allowing them to conclude that this was the reason for the effect.

Analysis and Conclusions

In quantitative research , the amount of data measured can be enormous. Data not prepared to be analyzed is called "raw data". The raw data is often summarized as something called "output data", which typically consists of one line per subject (or item). A cell of the output data is, for example, an average of an effect in many trials for a subject. The output data is used for statistical analysis, e.g. significance tests, to see if there really is an effect.

The aim of an analysis is to draw a conclusion , together with other observations. The researcher might generalize the results to a wider phenomenon, if there is no indication of confounding variables "polluting" the results.

If the researcher suspects that the effect stems from a different variable than the independent variable, further investigation is needed to gauge the validity of the results. An experiment is often conducted because the scientist wants to know if the independent variable is having any effect upon the dependent variable. Variables correlating are not proof that there is causation .

Experiments are more often of quantitative nature than qualitative nature, although it happens.

Examples of Experiments

This website contains many examples of experiments. Some are not true experiments , but involve some kind of manipulation to investigate a phenomenon. Others fulfill most or all criteria of true experiments.

Here are some examples of scientific experiments:

Social Psychology

  • Stanley Milgram Experiment - Will people obey orders, even if clearly dangerous?
  • Asch Experiment - Will people conform to group behavior?
  • Stanford Prison Experiment - How do people react to roles? Will you behave differently?
  • Good Samaritan Experiment - Would You Help a Stranger? - Explaining Helping Behavior
  • Law Of Segregation - The Mendel Pea Plant Experiment
  • Transforming Principle - Griffith's Experiment about Genetics
  • Ben Franklin Kite Experiment - Struck by Lightning
  • J J Thomson Cathode Ray Experiment
  • Psychology 101
  • Flags and Countries
  • Capitals and Countries

Oskar Blakstad (Jul 10, 2008). Experimental Research. Retrieved Aug 21, 2024 from Explorable.com: https://explorable.com/experimental-research

You Are Allowed To Copy The Text

The text in this article is licensed under the Creative Commons-License Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) .

This means you're free to copy, share and adapt any parts (or all) of the text in the article, as long as you give appropriate credit and provide a link/reference to this page.

That is it. You don't need our permission to copy the article; just include a link/reference back to this page. You can use it freely (with some kind of link), and we're also okay with people reprinting in publications like books, blogs, newsletters, course-material, papers, wikipedia and presentations (with clear attribution).

Want to stay up to date? Follow us!

Get all these articles in 1 guide.

Want the full version to study at home, take to school or just scribble on?

Whether you are an academic novice, or you simply want to brush up your skills, this book will take your academic writing skills to the next level.

example of research experimental

Download electronic versions: - Epub for mobiles and tablets - For Kindle here - For iBooks here - PDF version here

Save this course for later

Don't have time for it all now? No problem, save it as a course and come back to it later.

Footer bottom

  • Privacy Policy

example of research experimental

  • Subscribe to our RSS Feed
  • Like us on Facebook
  • Follow us on Twitter

Experimental Research

  • First Online: 25 February 2021

Cite this chapter

example of research experimental

  • C. George Thomas 2  

4830 Accesses

Experiments are part of the scientific method that helps to decide the fate of two or more competing hypotheses or explanations on a phenomenon. The term ‘experiment’ arises from Latin, Experiri, which means, ‘to try’. The knowledge accrues from experiments differs from other types of knowledge in that it is always shaped upon observation or experience. In other words, experiments generate empirical knowledge. In fact, the emphasis on experimentation in the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries for establishing causal relationships for various phenomena happening in nature heralded the resurgence of modern science from its roots in ancient philosophy spearheaded by great Greek philosophers such as Aristotle.

The strongest arguments prove nothing so long as the conclusions are not verified by experience. Experimental science is the queen of sciences and the goal of all speculation . Roger Bacon (1214–1294)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Bibliography

Best, J.W. and Kahn, J.V. 1993. Research in Education (7th Ed., Indian Reprint, 2004). Prentice–Hall of India, New Delhi, 435p.

Google Scholar  

Campbell, D. and Stanley, J. 1963. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. In: Gage, N.L., Handbook of Research on Teaching. Rand McNally, Chicago, pp. 171–247.

Chandel, S.R.S. 1991. A Handbook of Agricultural Statistics. Achal Prakashan Mandir, Kanpur, 560p.

Cox, D.R. 1958. Planning of Experiments. John Wiley & Sons, New York, 308p.

Fathalla, M.F. and Fathalla, M.M.F. 2004. A Practical Guide for Health Researchers. WHO Regional Publications Eastern Mediterranean Series 30. World Health Organization Regional Office for the Eastern Mediterranean, Cairo, 232p.

Fowkes, F.G.R., and Fulton, P.M. 1991. Critical appraisal of published research: Introductory guidelines. Br. Med. J. 302: 1136–1140.

Gall, M.D., Borg, W.R., and Gall, J.P. 1996. Education Research: An Introduction (6th Ed.). Longman, New York, 788p.

Gomez, K.A. 1972. Techniques for Field Experiments with Rice. International Rice Research Institute, Manila, Philippines, 46p.

Gomez, K.A. and Gomez, A.A. 1984. Statistical Procedures for Agricultural Research (2nd Ed.). John Wiley & Sons, New York, 680p.

Hill, A.B. 1971. Principles of Medical Statistics (9th Ed.). Oxford University Press, New York, 390p.

Holmes, D., Moody, P., and Dine, D. 2010. Research Methods for the Bioscience (2nd Ed.). Oxford University Press, Oxford, 457p.

Kerlinger, F.N. 1986. Foundations of Behavioural Research (3rd Ed.). Holt, Rinehart and Winston, USA. 667p.

Kirk, R.E. 2012. Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioural Sciences (4th Ed.). Sage Publications, 1072p.

Kothari, C.R. 2004. Research Methodology: Methods and Techniques (2nd Ed.). New Age International, New Delhi, 401p.

Kumar, R. 2011. Research Methodology: A Step-by step Guide for Beginners (3rd Ed.). Sage Publications India, New Delhi, 415p.

Leedy, P.D. and Ormrod, J.L. 2010. Practical Research: Planning and Design (9th Ed.), Pearson Education, New Jersey, 360p.

Marder, M.P. 2011. Research Methods for Science. Cambridge University Press, 227p.

Panse, V.G. and Sukhatme, P.V. 1985. Statistical Methods for Agricultural Workers (4th Ed., revised: Sukhatme, P.V. and Amble, V. N.). ICAR, New Delhi, 359p.

Ross, S.M. and Morrison, G.R. 2004. Experimental research methods. In: Jonassen, D.H. (ed.), Handbook of Research for Educational Communications and Technology (2nd Ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey, pp. 10211043.

Snedecor, G.W. and Cochran, W.G. 1980. Statistical Methods (7th Ed.). Iowa State University Press, Ames, Iowa, 507p.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Kerala Agricultural University, Thrissur, Kerala, India

C. George Thomas

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to C. George Thomas .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Thomas, C.G. (2021). Experimental Research. In: Research Methodology and Scientific Writing . Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-64865-7_5

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-64865-7_5

Published : 25 February 2021

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-64864-0

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-64865-7

eBook Packages : Education Education (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Our websites may use cookies to personalize and enhance your experience. By continuing without changing your cookie settings, you agree to this collection. For more information, please see our University Websites Privacy Notice .

Neag School of Education

Educational Research Basics by Del Siegle

Experimental research.

The major feature that distinguishes experimental research from other types of research is that the researcher manipulates the independent variable.  There are a number of experimental group designs in experimental research. Some of these qualify as experimental research, others do not.

  • In true experimental research , the researcher not only manipulates the independent variable, he or she also randomly assigned individuals to the various treatment categories (i.e., control and treatment).
  • In quasi experimental research , the researcher does not randomly assign subjects to treatment and control groups. In other words, the treatment is not distributed among participants randomly. In some cases, a researcher may randomly assigns one whole group to treatment and one whole group to control. In this case, quasi-experimental research involves using intact groups in an experiment, rather than assigning individuals at random to research conditions. (some researchers define this latter situation differently. For our course, we will allow this definition).
  • In causal comparative ( ex post facto ) research, the groups are already formed. It does not meet the standards of an experiment because the independent variable in not manipulated.

The statistics by themselves have no meaning. They only take on meaning within the design of your study. If we just examine stats, bread can be deadly . The term validity is used three ways in research…

  • I n the sampling unit, we learn about external validity (generalizability).
  • I n the survey unit, we learn about instrument validity .
  • In this unit, we learn about internal validity and external validity . Internal validity means that the differences that we were found between groups on the dependent variable in an experiment were directly related to what the researcher did to the independent variable, and not due to some other unintended variable (confounding variable). Simply stated, the question addressed by internal validity is “Was the study done well?” Once the researcher is satisfied that the study was done well and the independent variable caused the dependent variable (internal validity), then the research examines external validity (under what conditions [ecological] and with whom [population] can these results be replicated [Will I get the same results with a different group of people or under different circumstances?]). If a study is not internally valid, then considering external validity is a moot point (If the independent did not cause the dependent, then there is no point in applying the results [generalizing the results] to other situations.). Interestingly, as one tightens a study to control for treats to internal validity, one decreases the generalizability of the study (to whom and under what conditions one can generalize the results).

There are several common threats to internal validity in experimental research. They are described in our text.  I have review each below (this material is also included in the  PowerPoint Presentation on Experimental Research for this unit):

  • Subject Characteristics (Selection Bias/Differential Selection) — The groups may have been different from the start. If you were testing instructional strategies to improve reading and one group enjoyed reading more than the other group, they may improve more in their reading because they enjoy it, rather than the instructional strategy you used.
  • Loss of Subjects ( Mortality ) — All of the high or low scoring subject may have dropped out or were missing from one of the groups. If we collected posttest data on a day when the honor society was on field trip at the treatment school, the mean for the treatment group would probably be much lower than it really should have been.
  • Location — Perhaps one group was at a disadvantage because of their location.  The city may have been demolishing a building next to one of the schools in our study and there are constant distractions which interferes with our treatment.
  • Instrumentation Instrument Decay — The testing instruments may not be scores similarly. Perhaps the person grading the posttest is fatigued and pays less attention to the last set of papers reviewed. It may be that those papers are from one of our groups and will received different scores than the earlier group’s papers
  • Data Collector Characteristics — The subjects of one group may react differently to the data collector than the other group. A male interviewing males and females about their attitudes toward a type of math instruction may not receive the same responses from females as a female interviewing females would.
  • Data Collector Bias — The person collecting data my favors one group, or some characteristic some subject possess, over another. A principal who favors strict classroom management may rate students’ attention under different teaching conditions with a bias toward one of the teaching conditions.
  • Testing — The act of taking a pretest or posttest may influence the results of the experiment. Suppose we were conducting a unit to increase student sensitivity to prejudice. As a pretest we have the control and treatment groups watch Shindler’s List and write a reaction essay. The pretest may have actually increased both groups’ sensitivity and we find that our treatment groups didn’t score any higher on a posttest given later than the control group did. If we hadn’t given the pretest, we might have seen differences in the groups at the end of the study.
  • History — Something may happen at one site during our study that influences the results. Perhaps a classmate dies in a car accident at the control site for a study teaching children bike safety. The control group may actually demonstrate more concern about bike safety than the treatment group.
  • Maturation –There may be natural changes in the subjects that can account for the changes found in a study. A critical thinking unit may appear more effective if it taught during a time when children are developing abstract reasoning.
  • Hawthorne Effect — The subjects may respond differently just because they are being studied. The name comes from a classic study in which researchers were studying the effect of lighting on worker productivity. As the intensity of the factor lights increased, so did the work productivity. One researcher suggested that they reverse the treatment and lower the lights. The productivity of the workers continued to increase. It appears that being observed by the researchers was increasing productivity, not the intensity of the lights.
  • John Henry Effect — One group may view that it is competition with the other group and may work harder than than they would under normal circumstances. This generally is applied to the control group “taking on” the treatment group. The terms refers to the classic story of John Henry laying railroad track.
  • Resentful Demoralization of the Control Group — The control group may become discouraged because it is not receiving the special attention that is given to the treatment group. They may perform lower than usual because of this.
  • Regression ( Statistical Regression) — A class that scores particularly low can be expected to score slightly higher just by chance. Likewise, a class that scores particularly high, will have a tendency to score slightly lower by chance. The change in these scores may have nothing to do with the treatment.
  • Implementation –The treatment may not be implemented as intended. A study where teachers are asked to use student modeling techniques may not show positive results, not because modeling techniques don’t work, but because the teacher didn’t implement them or didn’t implement them as they were designed.
  • Compensatory Equalization of Treatmen t — Someone may feel sorry for the control group because they are not receiving much attention and give them special treatment. For example, a researcher could be studying the effect of laptop computers on students’ attitudes toward math. The teacher feels sorry for the class that doesn’t have computers and sponsors a popcorn party during math class. The control group begins to develop a more positive attitude about mathematics.
  • Experimental Treatment Diffusion — Sometimes the control group actually implements the treatment. If two different techniques are being tested in two different third grades in the same building, the teachers may share what they are doing. Unconsciously, the control may use of the techniques she or he learned from the treatment teacher.

When planning a study, it is important to consider the threats to interval validity as we finalize the study design. After we complete our study, we should reconsider each of the threats to internal validity as we review our data and draw conclusions.

Del Siegle, Ph.D. Neag School of Education – University of Connecticut [email protected] www.delsiegle.com

example of research experimental

Experimental Research: Meaning And Examples Of Experimental Research

Ever wondered why scientists across the world are being lauded for discovering the Covid-19 vaccine so early? It’s because every…

What Is Experimental Research

Ever wondered why scientists across the world are being lauded for discovering the Covid-19 vaccine so early? It’s because every government knows that vaccines are a result of experimental research design and it takes years of collected data to make one. It takes a lot of time to compare formulas and combinations with an array of possibilities across different age groups, genders and physical conditions. With their efficiency and meticulousness, scientists redefined the meaning of experimental research when they discovered a vaccine in less than a year.

What Is Experimental Research?

Characteristics of experimental research design, types of experimental research design, advantages and disadvantages of experimental research, examples of experimental research.

Experimental research is a scientific method of conducting research using two variables: independent and dependent. Independent variables can be manipulated to apply to dependent variables and the effect is measured. This measurement usually happens over a significant period of time to establish conditions and conclusions about the relationship between these two variables.

Experimental research is widely implemented in education, psychology, social sciences and physical sciences. Experimental research is based on observation, calculation, comparison and logic. Researchers collect quantitative data and perform statistical analyses of two sets of variables. This method collects necessary data to focus on facts and support sound decisions. It’s a helpful approach when time is a factor in establishing cause-and-effect relationships or when an invariable behavior is seen between the two.  

Now that we know the meaning of experimental research, let’s look at its characteristics, types and advantages.

The hypothesis is at the core of an experimental research design. Researchers propose a tentative answer after defining the problem and then test the hypothesis to either confirm or disregard it. Here are a few characteristics of experimental research:

  • Dependent variables are manipulated or treated while independent variables are exerted on dependent variables as an experimental treatment. Extraneous variables are variables generated from other factors that can affect the experiment and contribute to change. Researchers have to exercise control to reduce the influence of these variables by randomization, making homogeneous groups and applying statistical analysis techniques.
  • Researchers deliberately operate independent variables on the subject of the experiment. This is known as manipulation.
  • Once a variable is manipulated, researchers observe the effect an independent variable has on a dependent variable. This is key for interpreting results.
  • A researcher may want multiple comparisons between different groups with equivalent subjects. They may replicate the process by conducting sub-experiments within the framework of the experimental design.

Experimental research is equally effective in non-laboratory settings as it is in labs. It helps in predicting events in an experimental setting. It generalizes variable relationships so that they can be implemented outside the experiment and applied to a wider interest group.

The way a researcher assigns subjects to different groups determines the types of experimental research design .

Pre-experimental Research Design

In a pre-experimental research design, researchers observe a group or various groups to see the effect an independent variable has on the dependent variable to cause change. There is no control group as it is a simple form of experimental research . It’s further divided into three categories:

  • A one-shot case study research design is a study where one dependent variable is considered. It’s a posttest study as it’s carried out after treating what presumably caused the change.
  • One-group pretest-posttest design is a study that combines both pretest and posttest studies by testing a single group before and after administering the treatment.
  • Static-group comparison involves studying two groups by subjecting one to treatment while the other remains static. After post-testing all groups the differences are observed.

This design is practical but lacks in certain areas of true experimental criteria.

True Experimental Research Design

This design depends on statistical analysis to approve or disregard a hypothesis. It’s an accurate design that can be conducted with or without a pretest on a minimum of two dependent variables assigned randomly. It is further classified into three types:

  • The posttest-only control group design involves randomly selecting and assigning subjects to two groups: experimental and control. Only the experimental group is treated, while both groups are observed and post-tested to draw a conclusion from the difference between the groups.
  • In a pretest-posttest control group design, two groups are randomly assigned subjects. Both groups are presented, the experimental group is treated and both groups are post-tested to measure how much change happened in each group.
  • Solomon four-group design is a combination of the previous two methods. Subjects are randomly selected and assigned to four groups. Two groups are tested using each of the previous methods.

True experimental research design should have a variable to manipulate, a control group and random distribution.

With experimental research, we can test ideas in a controlled environment before marketing. It acts as the best method to test a theory as it can help in making predictions about a subject and drawing conclusions. Let’s look at some of the advantages that make experimental research useful:

  • It allows researchers to have a stronghold over variables and collect desired results.
  • Results are usually specific.
  • The effectiveness of the research isn’t affected by the subject.
  • Findings from the results usually apply to similar situations and ideas.
  • Cause and effect of a hypothesis can be identified, which can be further analyzed for in-depth ideas.
  • It’s the ideal starting point to collect data and lay a foundation for conducting further research and building more ideas.
  • Medical researchers can develop medicines and vaccines to treat diseases by collecting samples from patients and testing them under multiple conditions.
  • It can be used to improve the standard of academics across institutions by testing student knowledge and teaching methods before analyzing the result to implement programs.
  • Social scientists often use experimental research design to study and test behavior in humans and animals.
  • Software development and testing heavily depend on experimental research to test programs by letting subjects use a beta version and analyzing their feedback.

Even though it’s a scientific method, it has a few drawbacks. Here are a few disadvantages of this research method:

  • Human error is a concern because the method depends on controlling variables. Improper implementation nullifies the validity of the research and conclusion.
  • Eliminating extraneous variables (real-life scenarios) produces inaccurate conclusions.
  • The process is time-consuming and expensive
  • In medical research, it can have ethical implications by affecting patients’ well-being.
  • Results are not descriptive and subjects can contribute to response bias.

Experimental research design is a sophisticated method that investigates relationships or occurrences among people or phenomena under a controlled environment and identifies the conditions responsible for such relationships or occurrences

Experimental research can be used in any industry to anticipate responses, changes, causes and effects. Here are some examples of experimental research :

  • This research method can be used to evaluate employees’ skills. Organizations ask candidates to take tests before filling a post. It is used to screen qualified candidates from a pool of applicants. This allows organizations to identify skills at the time of employment. After training employees on the job, organizations further evaluate them to test impact and improvement. This is a pretest-posttest control group research example where employees are ‘subjects’ and the training is ‘treatment’.
  • Educational institutions follow the pre-experimental research design to administer exams and evaluate students at the end of a semester. Students are the dependent variables and lectures are independent. Since exams are conducted at the end and not the beginning of a semester, it’s easy to conclude that it’s a one-shot case study research.
  • To evaluate the teaching methods of two teachers, they can be assigned two student groups. After teaching their respective groups on the same topic, a posttest can determine which group scored better and who is better at teaching. This method can have its drawbacks as certain human factors, such as attitudes of students and effectiveness to grasp a subject, may negatively influence results. 

Experimental research is considered a standard method that uses observations, simulations and surveys to collect data. One of its unique features is the ability to control extraneous variables and their effects. It’s a suitable method for those looking to examine the relationship between cause and effect in a field setting or in a laboratory. Although experimental research design is a scientific approach, research is not entirely a scientific process. As much as managers need to know what is experimental research , they have to apply the correct research method, depending on the aim of the study.

Harappa’s Thinking Critically program makes you more decisive and lets you think like a leader. It’s a growth-driven course for managers who want to devise and implement sound strategies, freshers looking to build a career and entrepreneurs who want to grow their business. Identify and avoid arguments, communicate decisions and rely on effective decision-making processes in uncertain times. This course teaches critical and clear thinking. It’s packed with problem-solving tools, highly impactful concepts and relatable content. Build an analytical mindset, develop your skills and reap the benefits of critical thinking with Harappa!

Explore Harappa Diaries to learn more about topics such as Main Objective Of Research , Definition Of Qualitative Research , Examples Of Experiential Learning and Collaborative Learning Strategies to upgrade your knowledge and skills.

Thriversitybannersidenav

example of research experimental

  • Voxco Online
  • Voxco Panel Management
  • Voxco Panel Portal
  • Voxco Audience
  • Voxco Mobile Offline
  • Voxco Dialer Cloud
  • Voxco Dialer On-premise
  • Voxco TCPA Connect
  • Voxco Analytics
  • Voxco Text & Sentiment Analysis

example of research experimental

  • 40+ question types
  • Drag-and-drop interface
  • Skip logic and branching
  • Multi-lingual survey
  • Text piping
  • Question library
  • CSS customization
  • White-label surveys
  • Customizable ‘Thank You’ page
  • Customizable survey theme
  • Reminder send-outs
  • Survey rewards
  • Social media
  • Website surveys
  • Correlation analysis
  • Cross-tabulation analysis
  • Trend analysis
  • Real-time dashboard
  • Customizable report
  • Email address validation
  • Recaptcha validation
  • SSL security

Take a peek at our powerful survey features to design surveys that scale discoveries.

Download feature sheet.

  • Hospitality
  • Academic Research
  • Customer Experience
  • Employee Experience
  • Product Experience
  • Market Research
  • Social Research
  • Data Analysis

Explore Voxco 

Need to map Voxco’s features & offerings? We can help!

Watch a Demo 

Download Brochures 

Get a Quote

  • NPS Calculator
  • CES Calculator
  • A/B Testing Calculator
  • Margin of Error Calculator
  • Sample Size Calculator
  • CX Strategy & Management Hub
  • Market Research Hub
  • Patient Experience Hub
  • Employee Experience Hub
  • NPS Knowledge Hub
  • Market Research Guide
  • Customer Experience Guide
  • Survey Research Guides
  • Survey Template Library
  • Webinars and Events
  • Feature Sheets
  • Try a sample survey
  • Professional Services

example of research experimental

Get exclusive insights into research trends and best practices from top experts! Access Voxco’s ‘State of Research Report 2024 edition’ .

We’ve been avid users of the Voxco platform now for over 20 years. It gives us the flexibility to routinely enhance our survey toolkit and provides our clients with a more robust dataset and story to tell their clients.

VP Innovation & Strategic Partnerships, The Logit Group

  • Client Stories
  • Voxco Reviews
  • Why Voxco Research?
  • Careers at Voxco
  • Vulnerabilities and Ethical Hacking

Explore Regional Offices

  • Survey Software The world’s leading omnichannel survey software
  • Online Survey Tools Create sophisticated surveys with ease.
  • Mobile Offline Conduct efficient field surveys.
  • Text Analysis
  • Close The Loop
  • Automated Translations
  • NPS Dashboard
  • CATI Manage high volume phone surveys efficiently
  • Cloud/On-premise Dialer TCPA compliant Cloud on-premise dialer
  • IVR Survey Software Boost productivity with automated call workflows.
  • Analytics Analyze survey data with visual dashboards
  • Panel Manager Nurture a loyal community of respondents.
  • Survey Portal Best-in-class user friendly survey portal.
  • Voxco Audience Conduct targeted sample research in hours.
  • Predictive Analytics
  • Customer 360
  • Customer Loyalty
  • Fraud & Risk Management
  • AI/ML Enablement Services
  • Credit Underwriting

example of research experimental

Find the best survey software for you! (Along with a checklist to compare platforms)

Get Buyer’s Guide

  • 100+ question types
  • SMS surveys
  • Financial Services
  • Banking & Financial Services
  • Retail Solution
  • Risk Management
  • Customer Lifecycle Solutions
  • Net Promoter Score
  • Customer Behaviour Analytics
  • Customer Segmentation
  • Data Unification

Explore Voxco 

Watch a Demo 

Download Brochures 

  • CX Strategy & Management Hub
  • The Voxco Guide to Customer Experience
  • Professional services
  • Blogs & White papers
  • Case Studies

Find the best customer experience platform

Uncover customer pain points, analyze feedback and run successful CX programs with the best CX platform for your team.

Get the Guide Now

example of research experimental

VP Innovation & Strategic Partnerships, The Logit Group

  • Why Voxco Intelligence?
  • Our clients
  • Client stories
  • Featuresheets

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH1 1

Definition, Examples and Types of Experimental Research Designs

SHARE THE ARTICLE ON

What is Experimental Research ?

Experimental research is a scientific methodology of understanding relationships between two or more variables. These sets consist of independent and dependent variables which are experimentally tested to deduce a correlation between such variables in terms of the nature and strength of such relation. Such assessment helps in deriving a cause and effect relationship and is even used for the purpose of hypothesis testing.

In such a mechanism , independent variables involved are adjusted to discover their impact on the dependent variables. The degree to which a change in the independent variables influences dependent variables is the basis of gauging the degree of strength. Such variations are recorded over a specific period of time to ensure that the conclusions drawn about the relationship are substantive and reliable enough to assist intelligent decision making.

Experimental research deals with quantitative data and its statistical analysis which makes the study extremely useful and accurate. It finds its usability in fields of psychology , social sciences , physical evaluation and academics and are time bound studies usually used for verification purposes.

Types of Experimental Research designs

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH1 2

1) Pre-experimental research design :

This is an observational research mechanism used to evaluate changes in a group or various groups of dependent variables after changing the independent variable values. This is the simplest form of experimental research used to assess the need for further inspection, if satisfactory results are not derived from the observations registered.

This can further be subdivided as :

  • One-shot Case Study Research Design: A post-test study relying only on a single set of variables for observational purposes.
  • One-group Pretest-posttest Research Design : This is a combination of pre and post tests that studies a single set of variables before and after the method of testing has been implemented.
  • Static-group Comparison: The total groups of variables gets divided into 2 sub-groups, one subjected to the testing while the other group remains as it as . Observations at the end of the testing reveal the contrast between the tested and the non-tested panel.

2) True experimental research :

This is a statistical approach to establish a cause and effect relationship within a variable set. The quantitative approach of this study makes it highly accurate. The assignment of test units and treatments takes place in a randomized manner.

Apart from this , it uses the availability of a control group along with an independent variable that can be manipulated to obtain the required results.

3) Quasi- experimental research design :

Quasi-experimental research design is a partial representation of true experimental research such that it seeks to establish a cause and effect relationship by manipulating an independent variable, the only difference being that it does not adhere to random distribution of participants into groups.

Thus , Quasi- Experimental research design is only applied to those situations where there is no relevance or possibility for random distribution.

New call-to-action

Some examples of Experimental Research design

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH1

Employee recruitment and screening 

The recruitment of an employee to an organization requires the employee to go through a rigorous selection procedure that filters the highly suited individuals for the job from the rest of the lot. A screening process is conducted that tests the skills , qualification , experience and knowledge of the applicants before going ahead with selecting the required number of people. The selected individuals are then recruited and trained with respect to the work to be done. Following this training , these individuals are then observed for a specific frame. At the end of this time period , employee appraisals take place which reviews the performance of the employee to identify the need for any improvement or if the employee is capable of handling extra work while maintaining the same level of performance and consistency levels.

This is a simple example of one group pretest posttest research design that assists the creation of a progressive work environment that provides the room for employees to grow along with pushing the organization towards achieving objectives in an efficient manner.

Impact of online tuitions

A group of students belonging to the same class and scoring the same grades in their first term exams are selected to try out a new e-tuition app as against their existing tuition classes. This sample of students are divided into two groups : one that switches to the online educational tuition app while the other group continues to attend their existing tuition classes. This study continues till the next examination cycle , as it observes the differences in the students ability to learn , grasp concepts and their general attitude towards the process of online learning . At the end of the study , the students belonging to both the groups give their term end examinations and the differences between the performance of the students are noted to contrast the teaching methods and effectiveness of online learning vs e-learning.

Such a study is an example of static group comparison that helps in comparing , analysing and establishing one of the alternatives as a viable choice under the current scenario.

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH1 3

Disadvantages of Experimental Research

  • The chances of error and bias being involved in experimental research are very high. The process of controlling independent variables to study changes in the dependent variables is highly prone to human error. Further , the results can even be skewed if the values are manipulated by the researcher.
  • It is a highly expensive, time consuming and cumbersome process to carry out a thorough experimental research procedure.
  • The observational nature of the pre-test experimental research study makes it a qualitative research mechanism that does not help in deriving substantive conclusions based on hard figures.
  • It can produce artificial results . It is important to factor in all independent variables that bring out variation in the dependent variables . Failing to do this may not reflect the true picture with reference to the strength of the relationship between the variables in consideration.
  • In certain situations, It is highly risky and can lead to ethical complications if treatment is not implemented carefully.

Methods of data collection

EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH1 4

1) Surveys :

Surveys are the easiest and the most commonly used data collection mechanism. Surveys help in achieving the coverage of all relevant areas of interest by framing a questionnaire to be filled out by the targeted respondent. This can be done physically , however , the attractions offered by the online research software allow for advance designing , distribution, collection , reporting and analysis of the information gathered. This provides a viable alternative that offers enhanced research procedures to be conducted in a swift and efficient manner.

Care needs to be taken while designing the survey as well as selecting the limited number of respondents who will assist the surveying organizations in finding answers to their research questions to fuel intelligent decision making.

2) Observation :

This method of data collection involves keeping a check on the variables under study to monitor changes and observe behaviour. It takes a long period of observation to make significant conclusions. This method also largely relies on the observer’s judgement and so is highly subjective.

3) Simulation :

Simulation replicates real life processes and situations to understand variables under consideration. The reliability of such a method heavily depends upon the accuracy with which the simulation has been created. This method finds its applicability in fields such as operational research which seeks to break down the whole idea to study narrow concepts involved. Simulations are an effective choice where accessibility and implementation are not feasible.

4) Experiments :

Experiments are carried out in a controlled environment such as a lab where influencing factors can be controlled. This also circles around field experiments, numerical and AI studies. The usage of computerized software makes data handling and management an easy task.

Experiments assist a comprehensive overview of the variables under the scope of the study. They are statistically compatible and so deliver substantive results which are objective in nature.

Market Research toolkit to start your market research surveys and studies.

Difference between experimental and non- experimental research

1) Experimental research focuses on understanding the nature of relationship between independent and dependent variables involved under a particular field of study. On the other hand , Non-experimental research is descriptive in nature and so , focuses on defining a process , situation or idea.

2) Experimental research provides the freedom to control external independent variables to decipher relationships, however , such a control mechanism is absent in Non- experimental research.

3) Experimental data does not make use of case studies and published works for establishing relationships while non-experimental research cannot be carried out using simulations.

4) Experimental research involves a scientific approach whereas such an approach is absent in non-experimental research due to the descriptive nature of the study.

  The 3 types of experimental designs are :

  • Pre- experimental research 
  • True experimental research 
  • Quasi- experimental research 

The study of the impact of different educational levels , experience and additional skills on the nature of jobs , salaries and the type of work environment is a simple example that can be used to understand experimental research.

  Experimental research is a methodology used to gauge the nature of relationship between the variables in consideration.

Experimental designs are written in terms of the hypothesis that a study tries to prove or the variables the research tries to study.

Explore Voxco Survey Software

Online page new product image3 02.png 1

+ Omnichannel Survey Software 

+ Online Survey Software 

+ CATI Survey Software 

+ IVR Survey Software 

+ Market Research Tool

+ Customer Experience Tool 

+ Product Experience Software 

+ Enterprise Survey Software 

We use cookies in our website to give you the best browsing experience and to tailor advertising. By continuing to use our website, you give us consent to the use of cookies. Read More

Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
hubspotutk www.voxco.com HubSpot functional cookie. 1 year HTTP
lhc_dir_locale amplifyreach.com --- 52 years ---
lhc_dirclass amplifyreach.com --- 52 years ---
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
_fbp www.voxco.com Facebook Pixel advertising first-party cookie 3 months HTTP
__hstc www.voxco.com Hubspot marketing platform cookie. 1 year HTTP
__hssrc www.voxco.com Hubspot marketing platform cookie. 52 years HTTP
__hssc www.voxco.com Hubspot marketing platform cookie. Session HTTP
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
_gid www.voxco.com Google Universal Analytics short-time unique user tracking identifier. 1 days HTTP
MUID bing.com Microsoft User Identifier tracking cookie used by Bing Ads. 1 year HTTP
MR bat.bing.com Microsoft User Identifier tracking cookie used by Bing Ads. 7 days HTTP
IDE doubleclick.net Google advertising cookie used for user tracking and ad targeting purposes. 2 years HTTP
_vwo_uuid_v2 www.voxco.com Generic Visual Website Optimizer (VWO) user tracking cookie. 1 year HTTP
_vis_opt_s www.voxco.com Generic Visual Website Optimizer (VWO) user tracking cookie that detects if the user is new or returning to a particular campaign. 3 months HTTP
_vis_opt_test_cookie www.voxco.com A session (temporary) cookie used by Generic Visual Website Optimizer (VWO) to detect if the cookies are enabled on the browser of the user or not. 52 years HTTP
_ga www.voxco.com Google Universal Analytics long-time unique user tracking identifier. 2 years HTTP
_uetsid www.voxco.com Microsoft Bing Ads Universal Event Tracking (UET) tracking cookie. 1 days HTTP
vuid vimeo.com Vimeo tracking cookie 2 years HTTP
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
__cf_bm hubspot.com Generic CloudFlare functional cookie. Session HTTP
Name Domain Purpose Expiry Type
_gcl_au www.voxco.com --- 3 months ---
_gat_gtag_UA_3262734_1 www.voxco.com --- Session ---
_clck www.voxco.com --- 1 year ---
_ga_HNFQQ528PZ www.voxco.com --- 2 years ---
_clsk www.voxco.com --- 1 days ---
visitor_id18452 pardot.com --- 10 years ---
visitor_id18452-hash pardot.com --- 10 years ---
lpv18452 pi.pardot.com --- Session ---
lhc_per www.voxco.com --- 6 months ---
_uetvid www.voxco.com --- 1 year ---

Have a language expert improve your writing

Run a free plagiarism check in 10 minutes, generate accurate citations for free.

  • Knowledge Base
  • Starting the research process
  • 10 Research Question Examples to Guide Your Research Project

10 Research Question Examples to Guide your Research Project

Published on October 30, 2022 by Shona McCombes . Revised on October 19, 2023.

The research question is one of the most important parts of your research paper , thesis or dissertation . It’s important to spend some time assessing and refining your question before you get started.

The exact form of your question will depend on a few things, such as the length of your project, the type of research you’re conducting, the topic , and the research problem . However, all research questions should be focused, specific, and relevant to a timely social or scholarly issue.

Once you’ve read our guide on how to write a research question , you can use these examples to craft your own.

Research question Explanation
The first question is not enough. The second question is more , using .
Starting with “why” often means that your question is not enough: there are too many possible answers. By targeting just one aspect of the problem, the second question offers a clear path for research.
The first question is too broad and subjective: there’s no clear criteria for what counts as “better.” The second question is much more . It uses clearly defined terms and narrows its focus to a specific population.
It is generally not for academic research to answer broad normative questions. The second question is more specific, aiming to gain an understanding of possible solutions in order to make informed recommendations.
The first question is too simple: it can be answered with a simple yes or no. The second question is , requiring in-depth investigation and the development of an original argument.
The first question is too broad and not very . The second question identifies an underexplored aspect of the topic that requires investigation of various  to answer.
The first question is not enough: it tries to address two different (the quality of sexual health services and LGBT support services). Even though the two issues are related, it’s not clear how the research will bring them together. The second integrates the two problems into one focused, specific question.
The first question is too simple, asking for a straightforward fact that can be easily found online. The second is a more question that requires and detailed discussion to answer.
? dealt with the theme of racism through casting, staging, and allusion to contemporary events? The first question is not  — it would be very difficult to contribute anything new. The second question takes a specific angle to make an original argument, and has more relevance to current social concerns and debates.
The first question asks for a ready-made solution, and is not . The second question is a clearer comparative question, but note that it may not be practically . For a smaller research project or thesis, it could be narrowed down further to focus on the effectiveness of drunk driving laws in just one or two countries.

Note that the design of your research question can depend on what method you are pursuing. Here are a few options for qualitative, quantitative, and statistical research questions.

Type of research Example question
Qualitative research question
Quantitative research question
Statistical research question

Other interesting articles

If you want to know more about the research process , methodology , research bias , or statistics , make sure to check out some of our other articles with explanations and examples.

Methodology

  • Sampling methods
  • Simple random sampling
  • Stratified sampling
  • Cluster sampling
  • Likert scales
  • Reproducibility

 Statistics

  • Null hypothesis
  • Statistical power
  • Probability distribution
  • Effect size
  • Poisson distribution

Research bias

  • Optimism bias
  • Cognitive bias
  • Implicit bias
  • Hawthorne effect
  • Anchoring bias
  • Explicit bias

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the “Cite this Scribbr article” button to automatically add the citation to our free Citation Generator.

McCombes, S. (2023, October 19). 10 Research Question Examples to Guide your Research Project. Scribbr. Retrieved August 21, 2024, from https://www.scribbr.com/research-process/research-question-examples/

Is this article helpful?

Shona McCombes

Shona McCombes

Other students also liked, writing strong research questions | criteria & examples, how to choose a dissertation topic | 8 steps to follow, evaluating sources | methods & examples, "i thought ai proofreading was useless but..".

I've been using Scribbr for years now and I know it's a service that won't disappoint. It does a good job spotting mistakes”

  • Open access
  • Published: 21 August 2024

Identification of circulating tumor cells captured by the FDA-cleared Parsortix ® PC1 system from the peripheral blood of metastatic breast cancer patients using immunofluorescence and cytopathological evaluations

  • Mariacristina Ciccioli   ORCID: orcid.org/0009-0000-2518-0878 1 ,
  • Kyukwang Kim 2 ,
  • Negar Khazan  2 ,
  • Joseph D Khoury 3 ,
  • Martin J Cooke 1 ,
  • M Craig Miller 4 ,
  • Daniel J O’Shannessy 4 ,
  • Anne-Sophie Pailhes-Jimenez 1 &
  • Richard G Moore 2  

Journal of Experimental & Clinical Cancer Research volume  43 , Article number:  240 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

Metrics details

Circulating Tumor Cells (CTCs) may serve as a non-invasive source of tumor material to investigate an individual’s disease in real-time. The Parsortix ® PC1 System, the first FDA-cleared medical device for the capture and harvest of CTCs from peripheral blood of metastatic breast cancer (MBC) patients for use in subsequent user-validated downstream analyses, enables the epitope-independent capture of CTCs with diverse phenotypes based on cell size and deformability. The aim of this study was to determine the proportion of MBC patients and self-declared female healthy volunteers (HVs) that had CTCs identified using immunofluorescence (IF) or Wright-Giemsa (WG) staining. Peripheral blood from 76 HVs and 76 MBC patients was processed on Parsortix ® PC1 Systems. Harvested cells were cytospun onto a charged slide and immunofluorescently stained for identification of CTCs expressing epithelial markers. The IF slides were subsequently WG-stained and analyzed for CTC identification based on morphological features of malignant cells. All testing was performed by operators blinded to the clinical status of each subject. CTCs were identified on the IF slides in 45.3% (≥ 1) / 24.0% (≥ 5) of the MBC patients (range = 0 – 125, mean = 7) and in 6.9% (≥ 1) / 2.8% (≥ 5) of the HVs (range = 0 – 28, mean = 1). Among the MBC patients with ≥ 1 CTC, 70.6% had only CK + /EpCAM- CTCs, with none having EpCAM + /CK- CTCs. CTC clusters were identified in 56.0% of the CTC-positive patients. On the WG-stained slides, CTCs were identified in 42.9% (≥ 1) / 21.4% (≥ 5) of the MBC patients (range = 0 – 41, mean = 4) and 4.3% (≥ 1) / 2.9% (≥ 5) of the HVs (range = 0 – 14, mean = 0). This study demonstrated the ability of the Parsortix ® PC1 System to capture and harvest CTCs from a significantly larger proportion of MBC patients compared to HVs when coupled with both IF and WG cytomorphological assessment. The presence of epithelial cells in subjects without diagnosed disease has been previously described, with their significance being unclear. Interestingly, a high proportion of the identified CTCs did not express EpCAM, highlighting the limitations of using EpCAM-based approaches.

Securing tissue biopsy samples from metastatic tumors, particularly from certain organ sites, is highly invasive and complex. Alternative approaches include collection of tumor material from more easily accessible bodily fluids such as blood or urine to assess various phenotypic and/or genotypic aspects of the tumor’s biology [ 1 , 2 ]. The minimally invasive collection of blood, also referred to as a liquid biopsy, offers the potential to characterize tumors at genetic, transcriptional and protein levels and allows the opportunity to perform routine, repeated characterizations for monitoring a patients’ disease status and developing effective personalized treatments [ 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 ].

Circulating Tumor Cells (CTCs) are cells shed by solid tumors that migrate into the blood stream and disseminate. CTCs may extravasate through the endothelial cell layer into different tissues to form metastases in distant organs [ 7 , 8 , 9 ]. It is well known that CTCs can be used to predict disease progression and overall survival in patients with metastatic breast cancer (MBC) [ 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 ] and represent a reliable surrogate marker of treatment response and a potential alternative form of non-invasive monitoring of response to therapy [ 16 , 17 ]. Deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA), Ribonucleic acid (RNA) and proteins can be obtained from viable CTCs isolated from peripheral blood, offering invaluable insights into the biology of a cancer. Until recently, however, the process of isolating CTCs from blood was very challenging, limiting their routine use in the clinical setting [ 18 , 19 , 20 ].

CTCs are rare, representing an extremely small fraction of the cells present in a blood sample. Among the technologies developed to isolate CTCs, the CELLSEARCH® System (Menarini Silicon BioSystems) [ 21 ] is Food and Drug Administration (FDA)-cleared for CTC enumeration only. This method involves immune-affinity separation using antibodies against the Epithelial Cell Adhesion Molecule (EpCAM), leading to the selective isolation of a particular CTC phenotype that is likely not completely representative of all the cells being shed from the tumor and can potentially impact outcomes of gene expression analyses [ 22 , 23 , 24 , 25 ].

ANGLE developed the Parsortix ® PC1 System, a semi-automated device capable of capturing and harvesting CTCs from bodily fluids based on cell size and lack of deformability. The isolation/capture mechanism employed is purely physical, rather than epitope-dependent, allowing the system to capture cells with a variety of different phenotypes, such as epithelial or mesenchymal. The system employs a separation cassette (GEN3P6.5) containing a microfluidic structure comprising a series of steps across which cells flow, leading to a smaller critical gap. Most of the common blood cells and components (i.e. red blood cells (RBCs), white blood cells (WBCs), and platelets) pass across the critical gap, while CTC are retained in the separation cassette due to their size and rigidity, together with a small number of residual WBCs [ 26 , 27 ].

The observational study reported in this manuscript, referred to hereinafter as the ANG-008 study, was designed to demonstrate that the Parsortix ® PC1 System could capture and harvest CTCs from the peripheral blood of patients with MBC and that the CTCs harvested by the System could be used for subsequent downstream evaluation with immunofluorescence (IF) and cytology evaluations.

The following objectives defined in the ANG-008 clinical study are detailed in this manuscript:

Determine the proportion of MBC patients and female healthy volunteers (HVs or controls) that had one or more observable epithelial CTCs (as determined by IF) harvested from their peripheral blood using the Parsortix ® PC1 System.

Determine the proportion of MBC patients and female healthy volunteers (HVs or controls) that had one or more observable CTCs (as determined by cytomorphological review of the IF-stained slides that have been Wright-Giemsa (WG) stained) harvested from their peripheral blood using the Parsortix ® PC1 System and compare these results to the IF results.

The data generated from the ANG-008 study was used to support a De Novo request (DEN200062) for re-classification of the Parsortix® PC1 System as a Class II prescription device for use in MBC patients to capture and harvest CTCs for subsequent, user-validated, downstream evaluation, which was granted by the FDA on May 24, 2022 [ 28 ].

Materials and methods

Ethical conduct of the study.

The ANG-008 study was sponsored by ANGLE Europe Ltd (ANGLE) and involved the collection of whole blood samples from healthy women as well as from women with metastatic breast cancer. The study was considered to be exempt from the IDE (investigational device exemption) regulations (21 Code of Federal Regulations (CFR) Part 812.2.c.3) due to the fact that the only procedure required for participation in the study was the collection of blood samples, which is considered to be non-invasive, as well as the fact that none of the results of the research testing were reported back to the subjects and/or the investigators, or used in the diagnosis, treatment and/or care of the subjects.

This study was conducted in a manner consistent with:

United States (US) standards of Good Clinical Practice (GCP) as defined in US FDA CFR, particularly 21 CFR Part 812 (i.e. Sponsor & Investigator responsibilities), Part 50 (Informed Consent Requirements), Part 54 (Financial Disclosure), Part 56 (IRB Approval) and Part 11 (Electronic Records);

International GCP standards using the International Conference on Harmonization (ICH) guidelines on GCP;

Applicable FDA regulations;

Institutional Review Board(s) (IRB) requirements.

Sample size calculation

Based on the preliminary data and literature review, the hypothesis that the Parsortix ® PC1 System would be able to harvest observable CTCs as identified by IF in ≥ 25% of the MBC patients and in ≤ 3% of the HV control group was used. Assuming an overall study failure rate of ~ 5% (e.g., ineligible subjects, insufficient volume of blood, processing failures, etc.), it was expected that ~ 80 HVs and ~ 80 MBC patients would need to be enrolled to ensure a minimum of 75 HV subjects and 75 MBC patients with evaluable IF results for evaluation of the objective.

With a sample size of N = 75 evaluable MBC patients, a two-sided 95% confidence interval (CI) for a single proportion, using the large sample normal approximation, will extend a maximum of ± 11.4% from the actual proportion of MBC patients found to have observable CTCs. With a sample size of N = 75 evaluable HVs, a two-sided 95% CI for a single proportion, using the large sample normal approximation, will extend a maximum of ± 3.9% from the actual proportion of HVs with observable CTCs for an expected proportion of ≤ 3%.

Additionally, with a sample size of 75 evaluable MBC patients and 75 HVs, a two-group continuity corrected chi-square test with a 0.05 two-sided significance level (α) will have ~ 95% power (1—β) to detect a difference between a proportion of > 25% for MBC patients with observable CTCs and a proportion of < 3% for HV subjects with observable CTCs.

Enrollment and sample collection

All study participants provided informed consent before being enrolled in the study. Each subject was only entered into the study once. All laboratory testing was performed by operators blinded to the clinical status of the participants. A total of 76 female HVs and 85 MBC patients were enrolled between July 2019 and November 2019 at the clinical study site (University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY).

Inclusion criteria for the MBC patients were as follows:

Female ≥ 22 years of age;

Documented evidence of metastatic breast cancer (i.e. primary tumor histopathology of breast cancer and documented evidence of distant sites of metastasis by imaging, biopsy, and/or other means);

Willing and able to provide informed consent and agree to complete all aspects of the study.

The inclusion criteria for the HV subjects are detailed below. The information obtained from the HVs was ‘self-reported’, as complete medical records were not available at the enrolling site for these control subjects.

Females ≥ 22 years of age;

No known fever or active infections at the time of the blood collection;

No known current diagnosis of acute inflammatory disease or chronic inflammation;

No known current and/or prior history of malignancy, excluding skin cancers (squamous cell or basal cell);

Willing and able to provide informed consent and agrees to complete all aspects of the study.

None (0%) of the 76 HVs were found to be ineligible. A total of 9 (10.5%) of the 85 MBC patients enrolled were found to be ineligible or not usable for the study, leaving a total of 76 eligible MBC patients (Fig. 1 ).

figure 1

CONSORT Diagram for ANG-008 Study Subject Eligibility. Diagram shows enrolled patients, reasons for ineligibility within the MBC group, and breakdown of MBC patients in newly diagnosed, stable/responding diseases and progressive/recurrent disease groups

Four tubes of blood (one 3 mL K 2 Ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid (EDTA) vacutainer for CBC with differential and erythrocyte sedimentation rate testing, two 10 mL K 2 EDTA vacutainers for processing on the Parsortix ® System, and one 7.5 mL Serum-separating tube (SST) vacutainer for serum chemistry and lipid panel testing) were collected by venipuncture (or, for MBC patients, if applicable, through a venous port) from each HV subject and from each MBC patient, a minimum of seven days after the administration of a cytotoxic therapy (intravenously administered) and immediately prior to the administration of any other type of therapy. For the objectives detailed in this report, an average of 8.6 ± 1.2 mL of blood from one of the 10 mL K 2 EDTA vacutainers was processed on Parsortix ® PC1 Systems and the population of cells harvested were deposited onto cytology slides for cytopathological evaluation using IF and WG staining.

A breakdown of the ages, demographics, and clinical information for the eligible HV subjects and MBC patients is provided in Table 1 .

The demographics of the MBC patient population was consistent with the demographics of MBC patients described in the literature [ 29 ]. Approximately one-third of the MBC patients enrolled had progressive / recurrent metastatic disease (35.7%), 7.9% had newly diagnosed disease, with the largest proportion having stable/responding disease (57.9%). The race distribution is typical of most US based clinical trials, with the majority of patients having a white background. The breast cancer phenotype for most of the MBC patients was Estrogen Receptor (ER) /Progesterone Receptor (PR) positive and Human epidermal growth factor receptor-2 (HER2) negative, with approximately 89% being ER and/or PR positive and 21.1% having HER2 positive breast cancer. Bone was the most prevalent site of metastatic disease (67.1%), followed by the lymph nodes (26.3%), the liver (19.7%) and/or the lungs (18.4%), which are the most common sites of breast cancer metastasis reported in the literature [ 30 , 31 ]. There was a significant difference observed between the age and menopausal status of the HV subjects compared to the MBC patients, as the majority of the HV subjects were much younger compared to the MBC patients. This also led to significantly lower proportions of HV subjects with comorbidities and those taking medications compared to the MBC patients.

Blood processing on Parsortix ® PC1 instrument

Blood separation was performed at the Targeted Therapeutics Laboratory at the Wilmot Cancer Institute within eight hours from blood draw using Parsortix ® PC1 Systems. The Parsortix ® PC1 System is a bench top laboratory instrument consisting of inbuilt computer, pneumatic and hydraulic components, and other electronics to control the instrument hardware and behavior. The Parsortix ® PC1’s proprietary application software runs a series of encrypted Protocol Files (Clean, Prime, Separate, and Harvest) to control the instrument fluidic and hydraulic components. The instrument utilizes a single use, non-sterile Parsortix ® GEN3 Cell separation cassette, containing precision molded separation structures with ‘step’ configurations. Whole blood flows along a series of channels under controlled and constant pressure conditions (99 mbar) to enable separation. The channel height progressively decreases at each step toward the final ‘critical gap’. As a result, in the case of blood, cells are captured in the critical gap based on their size and resistance to compression. The looped cassette layout is designed to maximize the width of separating steps, which is a key factor affecting separation capability and capture capacity, providing fluid paths with minimal resistance to liquid flow. The cassette layout is intentionally omni-directional such that during a separation, the sample always flows across the step structures and then through the critical gap. To harvest cells captured in the cassette, this flow is intentionally reversed to release the cells from the critical gap and step structures and flush them out of the cassette into another receptacle using a small volume of buffer (~ 210 µL).

Cytology slide preparation

The Targeted Therapeutics Laboratory prepared the cytology slides for shipment to ANGLE Guildford central laboratory where the IF evaluations were performed. Following separation and enrichment, captured cells were harvested into a 1.5 mL microfuge tube containing 60 μL of fetal bovine serum (FBS). The harvested cells and FBS mixture was pipetted into a Cytospin® 4 Cytofunnel™ assembly (Thermo Fisher Scientific) containing a positively charged glass Shandon™ Single Cytoslides™ (Thermo Fisher Scientific). The slide assembly was cytocentrifuged at 800 rpm for 3 min on low acceleration, and the slide was removed from the assembly and allowed to air-dry at room temperature for 1 min. The air-dried slide was then submersed in ice-cold 100% acetone for 5 min at -20 °C and allowed to air-dry at room temperature for 30 min. The fixed slides were stored refrigerated (at + 2–8 °C) and shipped weekly to the ANGLE Guildford central laboratory for staining and evaluation.

Immunofluorescence staining and imaging

The development of the IF assay used in this study is described in Additional Files 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6. The procedure is summarized below.

Slides were kept refrigerated until IF staining was performed. Before staining, each slide was re-hydrated with 1 × Phosphate Buffered Saline (PBS) for 60 min. After re-hydration, slides were blocked with 2.5% Normal Horse Serum (S-2012 Vector Labs) and stained with an antibody mixture against surface blood lineage markers (CD45-Allophycocyanin (APC), CD16-APC, CD11b-APC and CD61-APC diluted in 1 × PBS) followed by another antibody mixture against intracellular markers (Cytokeratin (CK) 8-Alexa Fluor 488 (AF488), CK18-AF488, CK19-AF488, EpCAM-Alexa Fluor 555 (AF555), and 4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI) diluted in Inside Perm (Miltenyi Biotec)). Slides were mounted with 50 µL of 1 × PBS, a 25 mm × 25 mm glass coverslip and fixogum.

Slides were examined using a Leica LAS X fluorescence microscope or a BioView Allegro Plus imaging system, and the cells of interest (i.e. CTCs) were classified based on their staining patterns as follows: 1) EpCAM + , CK + , CD-, DAPI + ; 2) EpCAM + , CK-, CD-, DAPI + ; and 3) EpCAM-, CK + , CD-, DAPI + .

Wright-Giemsa staining and cytological evaluation

Upon completion of the IF evaluation, the coverslips were removed from each of the slides, and the slides were air dried and stored at room temperature until shipment to the Department of Hematopathology, Division of Pathology and Laboratory Medicine, at MD Anderson Cancer Center for WG staining and cytopathological evaluation. The slides underwent Richard-Allen Scientific WG staining on an automated stainer and examination by a qualified pathologist with expertise in blood evaluation and cytopathology (Dr. Joseph Khoury, JDK) using light microscopy. CTCs were identified and enumerated using conventional cytomorphologic criteria for malignancy, which included: size larger than peripheral WBCs, moderate to abundant cytoplasm, cytoplasmic vacuoles (micro or macro), irregular nuclear contours, nuclear hyperchromasia and prominent nucleoli. A CTC cluster was defined as 3 or more cohesive cells [ 32 ].

Immunofluorescence samples evaluation

Four of the 76 eligible HV subjects and one of the 76 eligible MBC patients had non-evaluable IF samples, leaving a total of 72 HV subjects and 75 MBC patients with evaluable IF stained slides. On the evaluable IF samples, four nucleated cells’ populations were identified: leukocytes (" Leukocytes " sect.), CTCs (" Circulating Tumor Cells " sect.), a cell population defined as other non-typical circulating cells (" Other Non-Typical Circulating Cells " sect.) and nucleated cells that remained unstained, i.e. negative for the epithelial, mesenchymal and CD markers included in the IF panel (" Unstained Cells " sect.). The breakdown of each cell population is shown in Fig. 2 .

figure 2

Breakdown of cell populations present in the harvests of HV and MBC subjects. Histograms show mean ± Standard Error of the Mean (SEM) of the ( A ) percentage and ( B ) absolute number of cells in each population (Multiple Mann Whitney test, * P  < 0.05, nd = discovery non-significant). Mean number is noted on each column

All cells that were nucleated, negative for CK/EpCAM, positive for CD markers and smaller than 20 µm in diameter were considered leukocytes. Since all four CD markers (CD45, CD16, CD11b and CD61) were combined and detected under one fluorescence channel, it was not possible to specify the subtypes of cells in this population. While leukocytes represented 98% of the nucleated cells identified on the MBC patients’ slides (mean: 1,908 cells per slide) and 99% of the cells identified on the HVs’ slides (mean: 1,367 cells per slide), it is important to note that the Parsortix ® PC1 System was highly efficient in enriching CTCs and eliminating the blood cells component from the starting blood samples, with a purity of > 99%, calculated as a percentage of the mean WBC difference before and after processing over the mean WBC count before processing: (Fig. 3 ).

figure 3

Performance of Parsortix ® PC1 System in eliminating leukocytes from whole blood for CTCs enrichment. A Histogram shows mean ± SEM of the number of WBCs present in the blood samples before enrichment vs the number of harvested WBCs ( p  ≥ 0.0001, Paired t-test) after enrichment for each donor. Graph includes all MBC and HV subjects with initial evaluable CBC count; B Table shows descriptive statistics

  • Circulating tumor cells

Only DAPI + cells that were also CD- were further evaluated for expression of CKs and/or EpCAM. CTCs were defined as cells that were DAPI + , CD- and CK + and/or EpCAM + . The results of the IF evaluation are summarized in Fig. 4 . Out of the 75 MBC patients with evaluable IF results, 41 (54.7%, Wilson 95% CI = 43.5% – 65.6%) had no cells classified as being epithelial CTCs, whereas 34 (45.3%, Wilson 95% CI = 34.5% – 56.6%) had one or more cells observed on their IF slides that were DAPI + , EpCAM + and/or CK + , and CD-, while 18 (24.0%, Wilson 95% CI = 15.8% – 34.8%) had five or more cells observed on their IF slides that were DAPI + , EpCAM + and/or CK + , and CD-. In the 34 MBC patients with one or more epithelial CTCs observed, 70.6% had only CK + , EpCAM- cells while the remaining 29.4% had ≥ 1 CK + , EpCAM + cell. No EpCAM + , CK- CTCs were identified in MBC patients. Among the CTC-positive MBC patients, clusters of CTCs, defined as two or more individual CTCs co-aggregating with or without the presence of leukocytes, were identified in 19 MBC patients (56%), with a range of 1-8 clusters per patient and a range of 2-44 CTCs per cluster. Of the 19 patients with CTCs clusters, 11 (58%) had at least one heterogeneous cluster defined as an aggregation of ≥ 2 CTCs and ≥ 1 leukocyte.

figure 4

IF evaluation results. A Representative images of CK+, EpCAM +/- CTCs and CTCs clusters identified in MBC patients and HV subjects (CKs-AF488) in green, EpCAM-AF555 in orange, Blood lineage markers (APC) in red, Nucleus (DAPI) in blue). B Dot plot shows median ± 95% CI of the number of CTCs identified in each MBC and HV donor by IF. A statistically higher number of CTCs was found in MBC patients compared to HVs (p≥0.001, Median test). C Table shows number of donors included in each cohort (N), range, median and average number of CTCs identified within each cohort, and, using CTC thresholds of 0, ≥1, ≥2, ≥3, ≥4, ≥5 and ≥10 CTCs identified, the number and percentage of donors within each CTC category along with Wilson 95% CI’s for each proportion. The Fisher’s exact test p -values shown are for the comparison of the proportions of HV subjects and MBC patients with less than vs. greater than or equal to varying numbers of CTCs observed on the IF slides, and in each instance, a significantly higher proportion of MBC patients were CTC positive compared to the HV subjects

In the 72 HV subjects with evaluable IF results, 67 (93.1%, Wilson 95% CI = 84.9% – 97.1%) had no cells classified as being CTCs whereas 5 (6.9%, Wilson 95% CI = 3.5% – 15.2%) had one or more cells observed on their IF slides that were DAPI + , EpCAM + and/or CK + , and CD-. One of the five CTC-positive HVs had only CK + , EpCAM + cells, three had only CK + , EPCAM- cells, while the remaining donor had a combination of both. Among the five CTC-positive HV subjects, one had 28 CTCs (this subject was identified as being pregnant at the time of their blood collection), one had 8 CTCs and the remaining three had ≤ 5 CTCs on their IF stained slides.

Taken together, as shown in Fig. 4 , a significantly higher proportion of MBC patients were CTCs positive compared to the HV subjects using any cut off for CTCs from ≥ 1 to ≥ 10.

Table 2 below summarizes the proportions of HV subjects and MBC patients with cells observed on their IF stained cytology slides that were classified as CTCs within various demographical and clinical characteristic subgroupings. Although the number of MBC patients in this study was relatively small, it is interesting to note that:

A significantly increased proportion of MBC patients had one or more cells classified as epithelial CTCs in port collected blood samples compared to venous collected blood samples (≥ 1 CTC: 60.5% vs. 25.0%, respectively, p -value = 0.003; ≥ 5 CTC: 34.9% vs. 9.4%, respectively, p -value = 0.014).

A significantly increased proportion of MBC patients taking pain medications had one or more cells classified as epithelial CTCs compared to those not taking pain medications (≥ 1 CTC: 56.3% vs. 25.9%, respectively, p -value = 0.016), however this observation was not statistically significant when using a CTC positivity cut off of ≥ 5 CTCs ( p -value = 0.089).

Other non-typical circulating cells

A population of large DAPI + , CK + , CD ± cells with characteristic morphology was identified and classified as “other non-typical circulating cells”. When negative for CD markers, these cells were distinguished from CTCs based on their distinct morphology and CK staining pattern. Non-typical circulating cells accounted on average for 0.45% and 1.12% of all harvested cells in the HV and MBC patient samples, respectively. Two cell populations were identified in this group based on their distinct morphology (Fig. 5):

“Small” non-typical circulating cells: these cells presented with a low epithelial signal (CK +, EpCAM-negative) and were either positive (~ 80%) or negative (~ 20%) for blood lineage markers. The average diameter was ~ 30 µm, with a large nucleus and small cytoplasmic area, and co-localization between the epithelial and nuclear signals (Fig. 5 .A). These cells were found in both HVs and MBC patients at a similar rate. Small non-typical circulating cells accounted for 85% and 53% of all non-typical circulating cells found in the HVs and MBC patient samples in this study, respectively (Fig. 5 C).

“Large” non-typical circulating cells: these cells presented with a low positivity for epithelial markers (CK + , EpCAM-) and were positive for blood lineage markers. These cells were between ~ 40 µm and ~ 80 µm in diameter and usually had a large nucleus and a large cytoplasm (Fig. 5 .B). They made up 15% and 47% of all non-typical circulating cells found in the HVs and MBC patient samples in this study, respectively. They were found in both HV and MBC patient samples, but in statistically higher numbers in MBC patient samples ( p  ≤ 0.0001). Additionally, the percentage of donors presenting with large non-typical circulating cells was 2.5-fold higher in MBC patients (67%) compared to HV subjects (27%) (Fig. 5 C-D).

figure 5

Non-typical circulating cells. A Representative image of a small non-typical circulating cell. B Representative image of a large non-typical circulating cell. Images were taken using a BioView Allegro Plus imaging system with a 10 × objective lens (CKs-AF488 in green, EpCAM-AF555 in orange, Blood lineage markers-APC in red/white in the merge image, nucleus-DAPI in blue). C Dot plot showing mean ± SEM of the number of non-typical circulating cells in each category in the harvest of 72 HV donors and 75 MBC (Two-Way ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test, ****P ≤ 0.0001). D Table shows the percentage donors with ≥ 1 non-typical blood cell. E Dot plot showing mean ± SEM of the number of non-typical circulating cells of each type in the harvest of 75 MBC (Two-Way ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test, * P  ≤ 0.05) divided in patients receiving or not cytotoxic therapy. F Table shows median and range numbers of graph E

Additionally, a statistically significantly higher number of non-typical circulating cells (small and large) were present in MBC patients receiving cytotoxic therapy (Fig. 5 E-F) and a weak positive correlation between the number of megakaryocytes and the number of CTCs (Spearman’s r = 0.28; p = 0.01) was observed.

Unstained cells

A small number of cells were found in both HV and MBC subjects that were nucleated and not stained by any of the epithelial or leukocyte markers used in this study. They represented 0.37% of all cells in MBC samples (mean = 5 cells per donor) and 0.44% of all cells in HV samples (mean = 5 cells per donor), with no statistically significant difference between HVs and MBCs (Fig. 2). In both HV and MBC samples, 91.6% of these cells (0.34%—0.41% of all cells) were smaller than 20 µm in diameter. Based on the size, it is reasonable to assume that these cells are likely to be leukocytes not expressing any of the protein markers used in this study. In both HV and MBC samples, 8.4% of the unstained cells (around 0.03% of all cells) were larger than 40 µm. Morphologically, these cells looked similar to the non-typical circulating cells population.

Wright-Giemsa samples evaluation

Seven of the 76 eligible HV subjects and six of the 76 eligible MBC patients had non-evaluable WG samples, leaving a total of 69 HV subjects and 70 MBC patients being evaluable for the WG evaluation.

The purpose of the WG evaluation was to determine if the IF-stained slides containing the cells harvested by the Parsortix ® PC1 System could be re-stained using WG reagents and evaluated by a pathologist for the identification of malignant cells (CTCs) and to determine the proportions of MBC patients and HV subjects with one or more malignant cells (CTCs) harvested from their peripheral blood using the Parsortix ® PC1 System as determined by a pathologist.

The results of the WG evaluation are summarized in Fig. 6 . It was noted by the pathologist during the review of the WG-stained IF cytology slides that there was significant cellular damage observed on the majority of the slides, particularly in the WBCs and RBCs. In the 70 MBC patients with evaluable results from the review of their WG-stained slide, 40 (57.1%, Wilson 95% CI = 45.4% – 68.0%) had no cells classified as being CTCs whereas 30 (42.9%, Wilson 95% CI = 31.9% – 54.5%) had one or more cells observed that were classified as malignant, including 15 (21.4%, Wilson 95% CI = 13.4% – 32.3%) with five or more cells observed that were classified as malignant. In the 40 MBC patients with no malignant cells identified on their WG-stained slides, 14 (35%) had one or more cells classified as CTCs by IF analysis. In the 30 MBC patients with malignant cells identified on their WG-stained slides, 16 (53%) had one or more cells classified as CTCs by IF analysis. In the 69 HV subjects with evaluable results from the review of their WG-stained IF cytology slides, 66 (95.7%, Wilson 95% CI = 88.1% – 98.5%) had no cells classified as being malignant whereas 3 (4.3%, Wilson 95% CI = 1.5% – 11.9%) had one or more cells observed that were classified by the pathologist as being malignant and 2 (2.9%, Wilson 95% CI = 0.9% – 9.9%) had five or more cells observed that were classified as malignant. In the 66 HV subjects with no malignant cells identified by WG analysis, 4 (6%) had one or more cells classified as CTCs by IF analysis. Of the 3 HV subjects with malignant cells identified by WG analysis, none of them had any cells classified as CTCs by IF analysis.

figure 6

WG evaluation results. A Dot plot shows median ± 95% CI of the number of malignant cells (CTCs) identified in each MBC and HV donor by WG. A statistically higher number of CTCs were found in MBC patients compared to HVs (p ≥ 0.001, Median test). B Table shows number of donors included in each cohort (N), range, median and average number of CTCs identified within each cohort, and, using CTC thresholds of 0, ≥ 1, ≥ 2, ≥ 3, ≥ 4, ≥ 5 and ≥ 10 CTCs identified, the number and percentage of donors within each CTC category along with Wilson 95% CI’s for each proportion. The Fisher’s exact test p-values shown are for the comparison of the proportions of HV subjects and MBC patients with less than vs. greater than or equal to varying numbers of CTCs observed on the WG-slides, and in each instance up to ≥ 5 CTCs, a significantly higher proportion of MBC patients were CTCs positive compared to the HV subjects

Taken together, as shown in Fig. 6, a significantly higher proportion of MBC patients were CTCs positive compared to the HV subjects using any cut off for CTCs from ≥ 1 to ≥ 5.

Table 3 below summarizes the proportions of HV subjects and MBC patients with cells observed on their WG-stained IF cytology slides that were classified as CTCs within various demographical and clinical characteristic subgroupings. Similar to what was observed in the by IF analysis, a significantly higher proportion of MBC patients with one or more cells classified as malignant was observed in the port collected blood samples compared to the venous blood samples (≥ 1 CTC: 66.7% vs. 12.9%, respectively, p -value < 0.001; ≥ 5 CTC: 38.5% vs. 0.0%, respectively, p -value < 0.001). It was also found that a significantly greater proportion of MBC patients who reported being on a cytotoxic therapy had one or more cells classified as malignant compared to those not on a cytotoxic therapy (≥ 1 CTC: 63.3% vs. 27.5%, respectively, p -value = 0.004; ≥ 5 CTC: 40.0% vs. 7.5%, respectively, p -value = 0.002).

The ANG-008 study was designed to determine the proportion of HV subjects and MBC patients that have one or more CTCs harvested from a minimum of ≥ 5 mL of blood using the Parsortix ® PC1 System and identified using IF or cytology evaluations. Both downstream assays used in this study require deposition of the harvested cells onto a slide. As previously reported in the ANG-002 HOMING Clinical Study [ 27 ] and in Additional File 6, a substantial proportion of the cells harvested by the Parsortix ® PC1 System are not retained on the Cytospin™ slides when using cytocentrifugation. Unfortunately, this cell loss is an unavoidable limitation of any conventional centrifugation-based cytology slide preparation method, including the optimized method used in these studies. These observations must be kept in mind when evaluating the proportion of donors that have one or more observable tumor cells identified on their cytology slides. Given this large cell loss, it is possible that a larger proportion of MBC patients had CTCs present in their Parsortix ® PC1 System harvests, but these cells were simply not retained on the cytology slides. Other downstream analysis techniques (e.g. molecular evaluations) may be able to utilize cells captured by the Parsortix ® PC1 System that are harvested directly into a tube without subsequent manipulations of the harvested material that could potentially lead to cell losses.

Despite the significant cell losses caused by the cytocentrifugation method used for the preparation of the cytology slides, a significantly larger proportion of MBC patients had one or five or more cells identified as epithelial CTCs by IF (DAPI + , CD- and EpCAM + and/or CK + cells) compared to HV subjects. These results demonstrate that cells harvested by the Parsortix ® PC1 System can be evaluated using IF staining techniques and that only a very small proportion of the HV subjects had cells harvested by the Parsortix ® PC1 System that were identified as epithelial cells using IF evaluation. Interestingly, a high proportion of the CTCs identified did not express EpCAM, highlighting the limitations of using EpCAM-based approaches to capture CTCs. The significance of the circulating epithelial cells in the HV subjects is unknown, but other investigators using IF staining with similar targets have also shown a small proportion of subjects without disease having cells that appear to be of epithelial origin identified in their bloodstream [ 33 ]. It is interesting to note that the HV subject with 28 CTCs identified on their IF-stained cytology slide was pregnant at the time of their blood donation, with the intriguing possibility that the isolated CKs expressing cells could represent circulating fetal cells [ 34 , 35 , 36 ].

Epithelial CTC clusters were identified by IF in 56% of the MBC patients that had at least one cell classified as a CTC, indicating that the Parsortix ® PC1 System capturing process does not disrupt cells aggregates and biological adhesions, in contrast to other CTC detection apparatuses [ 32 , 37 , 38 , 39 ]. As clusters of CTCs and clusters of CTCs with leukocytes are found to have differential biological features [ 32 , 39 ], such as an enhanced survival and metastatic potential, the potential to harvest intact CTC clusters further expands the possible applications of the System for evaluation of prognosis, diagnosis and therapy of the metastatic cancer.

Additionally, by IF staining, it was also possible to further characterize the Parsortix ® PC1 Systems’ harvests. The filtration method used in the System was able to eliminate > 99.99% of the leukocytes present in the blood sample with a small number still retained. This is likely due to the varying sizes of leukocytes, with the larger and less compressible ones being captured in the critical gap of the separation cassette alongside CTCs, further highlighting the importance of using sensitive and specific downstream methods for discerning CTCs from leukocytes. Other non-typical circulating cells of interest were also identified. Based on their large size and a review of the literature [ 40 , 41 , 42 , 43 , 44 , 45 ], it is hypothesized that the smaller non-typical circulating cells are megakaryocytes that have released their platelets and, therefore, appeared naked/cytoplasm free while the larger cells are functioning megakaryocytes, which typically range up to 150 µm in diameter. A recently published non-interventional prospective study involving 59 patients with MBC showed that megakaryocytes (confirmed by immunocytochemistry staining with anti-CD61) were identified in Parsortix ® harvests of 52% of the MBC patients, corroborating the results presented in Additional File 4, showing that these cells are typically CD61 + , a marker (platelet glycoprotein IIIa) specific for the megakaryocytic lineage. Additionally, a similar weak positive correlation, like the one reported in our study, between the number of megakaryocytes and the number of CTCs (Pearson’s r = 0.416 (95% CI 0.179–0.608); p = 0.001) was reported in the literature [ 41 ]. Looking at correlations with demographic information, it was found that a statistically higher number of non-typical blood cells (when combining small and large) were present in donors receiving cytotoxic therapy. While it is not possible to accurately point out the cause of this increase, it was previously reported that cytotoxic drugs can alter thrombopoiesis within the bone marrow [ 46 ]. The clinical relevance of circulating megakaryocytes is still debated, with studies showing that high number of megakaryocytes correlated with poor survival in advanced prostate cancer [ 40 ]. This finding widens the scope of use of the Parsortix ® PC1 System, as the capture of megakaryocytes by the Parsortix ® PC1 System is justified by their large size, something not achievable using epitope-based CTC detection methods. Isolation of megakaryocytes by liquid biopsy may have a great impact on future research as more is learned about their role in cancer dissemination and correlation with CTCs, and clusters.

It was also possible for a pathologist to identify malignant cells (CTCs) by applying WG staining to the IF-stained slides, with a significantly larger proportion of MBC patients having one or five or more cells observed as being malignant on the WG-stained slides generated from their Parsortix ® PC1 System harvests compared to the HV subjects. These results demonstrate that cells harvested by the Parsortix ® PC1 System can be evaluated using another staining technique following IF staining; however, the discordance between the cells identified as epithelial CTCs by IF staining and the cells identified as being malignant on the same slides by WG staining should be noted. The cellular damage caused by the IF staining procedure is the most likely cause for the observed differences, but this needs to be investigated further. Alternatively, it is possible the discordance between the two methods is due to the lack of targeting of mesenchymal CTCs in the IF assay. These cells are not expected to express EpCAM and/or CK and would have been classified as DAPI + cells only, whereas the cytopathological review of the WG-stained cells would have allowed for their identification based on morphology.

Additional correlation analyses between CTCs and participants demographics showed that a significantly increased proportion of MBC patients had one or more cells classified as CTCs in port collected blood samples compared to the venous blood samples. This correlation was observed using both cytological evaluations and confirmed previously observed results obtained in the ANG-002 clinical study [ 27 ], where it was speculated that the increased CTC prevalence may be due to the fact that blood from a central port comes directly from the tumor without first filtering through additional capillary beds, while peripheral blood drawn from antecubital veins has likely circulated through both lung and peripheral capillaries after egressing from the tumor. Another possible hypothesis for this finding is that patients with a central port are usually receiving intravenous chemotherapy and, thus, may have a more aggressive disease compared to other MBC patients. The second correlation observed by IF was related to the use of pain medications. Due to the small sample size, nothing definitive can be concluded, however, it may be another association with more aggressive disease status. This correlation was not statistically significant when using a CTC threshold of ≥ 5 CTCs.

In addition to the previously detailed limitation about the use of cytocentrifugation, other limitations of this study were:

Set Volume of Blood Not Used: The ANG-008 clinical study specified that a minimum volume of blood needed to be available (≥ 5 mL) for the processing of each sample rather specifying that an exact volume of blood would be used for each sample. The primary reason for this was to reduce and minimize as much user intervention to the blood as possible (for example, decanting or pipetting an exact volume of blood into a separate vessel). Additionally, because the aim was not the enumeration of cells, but rather the capture and harvesting of cells for subsequent evaluation, it was felt that only a minimum volume of blood should be specified to demonstrate the feasibility of using different types of downstream analyses on the Parsortix ® System harvests. We recognize that the use of varying volumes of blood for each sample makes it more difficult to directly compare results between samples that used different volumes of blood. We also recognize that there is variability in the numbers of cells between different tubes (tube-to-tube variability).

Mesenchymal CTCs Not Assayed: The IF assay used in this study only included markers to target epithelial CTCs. However, it is known that tumor cells can undergo epithelial-to-mesenchymal transition (EMT) when entering the bloodstream to eventually establish distant metastases. Therefore, the inclusion of antibodies to target mesenchymal markers in the IF panel would maximize the information that can be obtained from each blood samples and potentially identify more clinically relevant CTCs for analysis. Nevertheless, while mesenchymal CTCs were not stained using the IF panel used in this study, it is possible to assume that EMT cells, with low/no expression of EpCAM and still retaining cytokeratins expression, were indeed detected. Additionally, as shown in Fig. 2, the percentage/number of nucleated unstained cells was comparable between HVs and MBC patients. While it is not possible to decipher the exact nature of these cells, the fact that comparable numbers are present in the healthy and patients’ cohorts indicates that the mesenchymal CTCs component was not very abundant in the patients included in this study, as compared to the already identified epithelial and EMT CTCs.

Clinical Utility Not Evaluated: The intention of the ANG-008 clinical study was to demonstrate that the Parsortix ® PC1 System could capture and harvest CTCs from the blood of metastatic breast cancer patients for subsequent analysis using IF and WG staining. The clinical utility of CTC enrichment in patients with MBC using the Parsortix ® PC1 System will need to be demonstrated in follow-up studies using validated downstream evaluation methods.

The IF staining process appeared to introduce a significant amount of cellular damage, making the cytopathological review of the WG-stained slides more difficult. Presumably this cellular damage was caused by the use of acetone as the cellular fixative as well as the IF staining process which required permeabilization of the cells due to the use of antibodies directed against intracellular targets. Fixation should be standardized to be able to successfully combine alternative staining methods on the same slide.

This study demonstrated that the population of cells captured and harvested using the Parsortix ® PC1 System could be evaluated using IF and WG staining, and that a significantly larger proportion of MBC patients had one or five or more cells defined as being malignant compared to HV subjects. Interestingly, a majority of the cells classified as epithelial CTCs by IF did not express EpCAM, further highlighting limitations of using EpCAM-based approaches to capture CTCs. It was also possible to identify not only individual CTCs, but also clusters of CTCs and other non-typical circulating cells of interest, demonstrating that the Parsortix ® PC1 System could potentially be utilized to bridge the gap in CTC clusters analysis and further expand the understanding of metastasis dissemination that can be obtained from a liquid biopsy.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

Alexa Fluor 488 dye

Alexa Fluor 555 dye

Allophycocyanin

Body Mass Index

Body surface area

Complete Blood Count (CBC)

Code of Federal Regulations

Confidence Interval

Circulating tumor cell

Cytokeratin

4′,6-Diamidino-2-phenylindole

Deoxyribonucleic acid

Ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid

Epithelial-Mesenchymal Transition

Epithelial Cell Adhesion Molecule

Estrogen Receptor

Fetal bovine serum

Food and Drug Administration

Good Clinical Practice

Human epidermal growth factor receptor-2

Healthy volunteer

International Conference on Harmonization

Immunofluorescence

  • Metastatic breast cancer

Phosphate Buffered Saline

Progesterone Receptor

Parsortix ® PC1 instrument designation

Red Blood Cell(s)

Ribonucleic acid

Standard Deviation

Standard Error of the Mean

Serum-separating tubes

United States

White blood cell(s)

Wright-Giemsa

Micron or micrometer

Poulet G, Massias J, Taly V. Liquid biopsy: general concepts. Acta Cytol. 2019;63:449–55. https://doi.org/10.1159/000499337 .

CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Costa JL, Schmitt FC. Liquid biopsy: a new tool in oncology. Acta Cytol. 2019;63:448. https://doi.org/10.1159/000501355 .

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Alix-Panabières C, Pantel K. Liquid biopsy: from discovery to clinical application. Cancer Discov. 2021;11:858–73. https://doi.org/10.1158/2159-8290.CD-20-1311 .

Alimirzaie S, Bagherzadeh M, Akbari MR. Liquid biopsy in breast cancer: a comprehensive review. Clin Genet. 2019;95:643–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/cge.13514 .

Ignatiadis M, Sledge GW, Jeffrey SS. Liquid biopsy enters the clinic — implementation issues and future challenges. Nat Rev Clin Oncol. 2021;18:297–312. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41571-020-00457-x .

Alix-Panabières C, Pantel K. Clinical applications of circulating tumor cells and circulating tumor DNA as liquid biopsy. Cancer Discov. 2016;6:479–91. https://doi.org/10.1158/2159-8290.CD-15-1483 .

Paoletti C, Hayes DF. Circulating tumor cells. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2016;882:235–58. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-22909-6_10 .

Cherdyntseva NV, Litviakov NV, Denisov EV, Gervas PA, Cherdyntsev ES. Circulating tumor cells in breast cancer: functional heterogeneity, pathogenetic and clinical aspects. Exp Oncol. 2017;39:2–11. https://doi.org/10.31768/2312-8852.2017.39(1):2-11 .

Pantel K, Alix-Panabières C. Crucial roles of circulating tumor cells in the metastatic cascade and tumor immune escape: biology and clinical translation. J Immunother Cancer. 2022;10: e005615. https://doi.org/10.1136/jitc-2022-005615 .

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Giuliano M, Giordano A, Jackson S, Hess KR, de Giorgi U, Mego M, et al. Circulating tumor cells as prognostic and predictive markers in metastatic breast cancer patients receiving first-line systemic treatment. Breast Cancer Res. 2011;13:13. https://doi.org/10.1186/bcr2907 .

Google Scholar  

Mu Z, Wang C, Ye Z, Austin L, Civan J, Hyslop T, et al. Prospective assessment of the prognostic value of circulating tumor cells and their clusters in patients with advanced-stage breast cancer. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2015;154:563–71. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10549-015-3636-4 .

Harigopal M, Kowalski D, Vosoughi A. Enumeration and molecular characterization of circulating tumor cells as an innovative tool for companion diagnostics in breast cancer. Expert Rev Mol Diagn. 2020;20:815–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/14737159.2020.1784009 .

Budd GT, Cristofanilli M, Ellis MJ, Stopeck A, Borden E, Miller MC, et al. Circulating tumor cells versus imaging–predicting overall survival in metastatic breast cancer. Clin Cancer Res. 2006;12:6403–9. https://doi.org/10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-05-1769 .

Hayes DF, Cristofanilli M, Budd GT, Ellis MJ, Stopeck A, Miller MC, et al. Circulating tumor cells at each follow-up time point during therapy of metastatic breast cancer patients predict progression-free and overall survival. Clin Cancer Res. 2006;12:4218–24. https://doi.org/10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-05-2821 .

Cristofanilli M, Budd GT, Ellis MJ, Stopeck A, Matera J, Miller MC, et al. Circulating tumor cells, disease progression, and survival in metastatic breast cancer. N Engl J Med. 2004;351:781–91. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMOA040766 .

de Freitas AJA, Causin RL, Varuzza MB, Calfa S, Hidalgo Filho CMT, Komoto TT, et al. Liquid biopsy as a tool for the diagnosis, treatment, and monitoring of breast cancer. Int J Mol Sci. 2022;23(17):9952. https://doi.org/10.3390/IJMS23179952 .

CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Zhang X, Ju S, Wang X, Cong H. Advances in liquid biopsy using circulating tumor cells and circulating cell-free tumor DNA for detection and monitoring of breast cancer. Clin Exp Med. 2019;19:271–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10238-019-00563-w .

Lianidou ES, Markou A. Circulating tumor cells in breast cancer: detection systems, molecular characterization, and future challenges. Clin Chem. 2011;57:1242–55. https://doi.org/10.1373/clinchem.2011.165068 .

Costa C, Dávila-Ibáñez AB. Methodology for the isolation and analysis of CTCs. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2020;1220:45–59. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-35805-1_4 .

Hu X, Zang X, Lv Y. Detection of circulating tumor cells: advances and critical concerns. Oncol Lett. 2021;21:422. https://doi.org/10.3892/ol.2021.12683 .

Riethdorf S, O’Flaherty L, Hille C, Pantel K. Clinical applications of the cell search platform in cancer patients. Adv Drug Deliv Rev. 2018;125:102–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ADDR.2018.01.011 .

Xu L, Mao X, Imrali A, Syed F, Mutsvangwa K, Berney D, et al. Optimization and evaluation of a novel size based circulating tumor cell isolation system. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0138032.

Andree KC, van Dalum G, Terstappen LWMM. Challenges in circulating tumor cell detection by the cell search system. Mol Oncol. 2016;10:395–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.MOLONC.2015.12.002 .

Topa J, Grešner P, Żaczek AJ, Markiewicz A. Breast cancer circulating tumor cells with mesenchymal features-an unreachable target? Cell Mol Life Sci. 2022;79:79. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00018-021-04064-6 .

Gorges TM, Tinhofer I, Drosch M, Röse L, Zollner TM, Krahn T, et al. Circulating tumour cells escape from EpCAM-based detection due to epithelial-to-mesenchymal transition. BMC Cancer. 2012;12:12. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2407-12-178 .

CAS   Google Scholar  

Miller MC, Robinson PS, Wagner C, O’Shannessy DJ. The Parsortix™ cell separation system-a versatile liquid biopsy platform. Cytometry A. 2018;93:1234–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/CYTO.A.23571 .

Cohen EN, Jayachandran G, Moore RG, Cristofanilli M, Lang JE, Khoury JD, et al. A multi-center clinical study to harvest and characterize circulating tumor cells from patients with metastatic breast cancer using the Parsortix® PC1 system. Cancers (Basel). 2022;14: 5238. https://doi.org/10.3390/CANCERS14215238 .

DEN200062 Trade/Device Name: Parsortix® PC1 Device Regulation Number: 21 CFR 866.6110 Regulation Name: Circulating tumor cell enrichment device Regulatory Class: Class II Product Code: QSA Dated: September 25, 2020 Received: September 28, 2020 n.d.

Breast cancer facts & figures 2019–2020. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/174_2016_83 .

Hess KR, Varadhachary GR, Taylor SH, Wei W, Raber MN, Lenzi R, et al. Metastatic patterns in adenocarcinoma. Cancer. 2006;106:1624–33. https://doi.org/10.1002/cncr.21778 .

Soni A, Ren Z, Hameed O, Chanda D, Morgan CJ, Siegal GP, et al. Breast cancer subtypes predispose the site of distant metastases. Am J Clin Pathol. 2015;143:471–8. https://doi.org/10.1309/AJCPYO5FSV3UPEXS .

Piñeiro R, Martínez-Pena I, López-López R. Relevance of CTC clusters in breast cancer metastasis. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2020;1220:93–115. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-35805-1_7 .

Jin L, Zhao W, Zhang J, Chen W, Xie T, Wang L, et al. Evaluation of the diagnostic value of circulating tumor cells with CytoSorter® CTC capture system in patients with breast cancer. Cancer Med. 2020;9:1638–47. https://doi.org/10.1002/cam4.2825 .

Seppo A, Frisova V, Ichetovkin I, Kim Y, Evans MI, Antsaklis A, et al. Detection of circulating fetal cells utilizing automated microscopy: potential for noninvasive prenatal diagnosis of chromosomal aneuploidies. Prenat Diagn. 2008;28:815–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/pd.1987 .

Hatt L, Brinch M, Singh R, Møller K, Lauridsen RH, Schlütter JM, et al. A new marker set that identifies fetal cells in maternal circulation with high specificity. Prenat Diagn. 2014;34:1066–72. https://doi.org/10.1002/PD.4429 .

Mergenthaler S, Babochkina T, Kiefer V, Lapaire O, Holzgreve W, Hahn S. FISH analysis of all fetal nucleated cells in maternal whole blood: Improved specificity by the use of two Y-chromosome probes. J Histochem Cytochem. 2005;53:319–22. https://doi.org/10.1369/jhc.4A6404.2005 .

Amintas S, Bedel A, Moreau-Gaudry F, Boutin J, Buscail L, Merlio JP, et al. Circulating tumor cell clusters: united we stand divided we fall. Int J Mol Sci. 2020;21:21. https://doi.org/10.3390/IJMS21072653 .

Hong Y, Fang F, Zhang Q. Circulating tumor cell clusters: what we know and what we expect (review). Int J Oncol. 2016;49:2206. https://doi.org/10.3892/IJO.2016.3747 .

Herath S, Razavi Bazaz S, Monkman J, Ebrahimi Warkiani M, Richard D, O’Byrne K, et al. Circulating tumor cell clusters: insights into tumour dissemination and metastasis. Expert Rev Mol Diagn. 2020;20:1139–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/14737159.2020.1846523 .

Xu L, Mao X, Guo T, Chan PY, Shaw G, Hines J, et al. The novel association of circulating tumor cells and circulating megakaryocytes with prostate cancer prognosis. Clin Cancer Res. 2017;23:5112–22. https://doi.org/10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-16-3081 .

Grašič Kuhar C, Silvester J, Mencinger M, Ovčariček T, Čemažar M, Miceska S, et al. Association of circulating tumor cells, megakaryocytes and a high immune-inflammatory environment in metastatic breast cancer. Cancers (Basel). 2023;15:3397. https://doi.org/10.3390/CANCERS15133397/S1 .

Kanikarla-Marie P, Lam M, Menter DG, Kopetz S. Platelets, circulating tumor cells, and the circulome. Cancer Metastasis Rev. 2017;36:235–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10555-017-9681-1 .

Machlus KR, Italiano JE. The incredible journey: from megakaryocyte development to platelet formation. J Cell Biol. 2013;201:785–96. https://doi.org/10.1083/jcb.201304054 .

Thon JN, Italiano JE. Platelets: production, morphology and ultrastructure. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2012;210:3–22. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-29423-5_1 .

Scott GBD. Circulating megakaryocytes. Histopathology. 1982;6:467–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1365-2559.1982.TB02743.X .

Bakchoul T, Marini I. Drug-associated thrombocytopenia. Hematology. 2018;2018:576–83. https://doi.org/10.1182/ASHEDUCATION-2018.1.576 .

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge and thank all the women who participated in the clinical studies and provided their blood samples, as well as the following individuals (previous employees of ANGLE Europe Limited, Study Sponsor) for their contribution to the conduct of the studies, processing of samples, and evaluation of results: Natalia Bravo-Santano for the design of Figure 4 and samples analysis, Victoria Hatton and Ofure Alenkhe for samples staining and analysis.

This work was fully funded by ANGLE Europe Limited.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

ANGLE Europe Limited, Guildford, UK

Mariacristina Ciccioli, Martin J Cooke & Anne-Sophie Pailhes-Jimenez

Division of Gynecologic Oncology, Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Wilmot Cancer Institute, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, USA

Kyukwang Kim, Negar Khazan  & Richard G Moore

Department of Pathology, Microbiology, and Immunology, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA

Joseph D Khoury

ANGLE North America, Plymouth Meeting, PA, USA

M Craig Miller & Daniel J O’Shannessy

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

RM, MJC, DJO and MCM contributed to the conception and design of the study. MC and ASPJ contributed to the development of the IF assay, staining, imaging, and analysis of the IF data. MCM, MC and ASPJ contributed to the statistical analysis and interpretation of the overall results. RM, KK, and NK contributed to samples collection and processing on Parsortix ® Systems. JDK analyzed and interpreted the cytology data. MC collated the data and drafted the final manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mariacristina Ciccioli .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The ANG-008 study was approved by the University of Rochester’s Office for Human Subject Protection Research Subject Review Board (FWA00009386, IRB reference STUDY00003288, CTO# IBRS18143), and all participants provided written informed consent prior to enrollment into the study. See Methods for more details.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Authors MC, MJC, MCM and ASPJ are employees of ANGLE. DJO was an employee at ANGLE. JDK has received honoraria as an ANGLE Scientific Advisor.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

13046_2024_3149_moesm1_esm.pdf.

Additional File 1. Mean analytical sensitivity and analytical specificity of the CK8/CK18/CK19-AF488 panel. (A) Dot plot shows mean ± SEM of the percentage analytical sensitivity of the CK panel in SKBR3 cells harvested from spiked blood of 20 healthy volunteers (N=20, mean=98%) separated through Parsortix ® instruments and percentage analytical specificity of the CK panel in Hs 578T cells harvested from spiked blood of 12 healthy donors (N=12, mean=100%) separated through Parsortix ® instruments. Only <1% (6/631) SKBR3 cells had a non-detectable CK signal indicating an overall analytical sensitivity of 99%. No Hs 578T cells had detectable CK signal, indicating an overall analytical specificity of 100%. (B) Representative image of a SKBR3 cell stained with the optimized panel. (C) Representative image of a Hs 578T cell stained with the optimized panel. Images were taken using a 10× objective on BioView Allegro Plus automated imaging system and are shown with 4× post imaging zoom. Merge colors: CD45/CD16/CD11b/CD61 (white), DAPI (blue), CK8/CK18/CK19 (green). Micron bar= 30 µm

13046_2024_3149_MOESM2_ESM.pdf

Additional File 2. Analytical sensitivity and analytical specificity of EpCAM-AF555. (A) Dot plot shows mean ± SEM of EpCAM MFI (normalized for imaging exposure) in SKBR3, CaOV3, Hs 578T and WBCs. Line represents MFI value indicating detectable signal. Data were obtained from cancer cells harvested from spiked blood of healthy volunteers separated through Parsortix ® instruments. Analytical specificity was 100% as detected by the absence of detectable signal in Hs 578T cells (0/13) and in leukocytes (0/199). Analytical sensitivity was assessed in SKBR3 and CaOV3 cells with only 2/34 SKBR3 cells below the detectable threshold, indicating analytical sensitivity of 94%, and 0/21 CaOV3 cells below the detectable threshold, indicating analytical sensitivity of 100%.  (B) Representative images of EpCAM-AF555 staining in Hs 578T cells, CaOV3 cells, SKBR3 cells and WBCs. Images taken using a 10× objective on BioView Allegro Plus automated imaging system and are shown with 4× post imaging zoom. Merge colors: CD45/CD16/CD11b/CD61 (white), DAPI (blue), CK8/CK18/CK19 (green), EpCAM (red). Micron bar= 30 µm

13046_2024_3149_MOESM3_ESM.pdf

Additional File 3. (A) Dot plot shows mean ± SEM of the percentage of harvested cells (excluding spiked cancer cells) stained by CD45 in 12 healthy volunteers’ harvests from Parsortix ® instruments. An average of 96% of the leukocytes found in healthy volunteers’ harvest samples expressed CD45. (B) Representative image of leukocytes stained by CD45 (APC, red) and DAPI (blue) and negative for CK8/CK18/CK19 (AF488, green) and EpCAM (AF555, orange). (C) Representative image of a SKBR3 cell stained by CK8/CK18/CK19 (AF488, green), EpCAM (AF555, orange) and DAPI (blue) and negative for CD45 (APC, red). Merge colors: CD45 (white), DAPI (blue), CK8/CK18/CK19 (green), EpCAM (red). Images taken with BioView Allegro Plus automated imaging system using a 10x objective lens and shown with 4× post imaging zoom. Micron bar= 30 µm

13046_2024_3149_MOESM4_ESM.pdf

Additional File 4. (A) Dot plot shows mean ± SEM of the absolute number of CK+, EpCAM+/-, CDs- cells found when using CD45 alone, in combination with CD16/CD11b and in combination with CD16/CD11b/CD61, with and without user morphological evaluation on the identified cells in Parsortix ® harvests of metastatic breast cancer and healthy volunteer (HV) subjects. (B) Table shows percentage of samples with at least 1 CK+, EpCAM+/-, CD- cell. (C) Histogram shows mean ± SEM of the percentage of other non-typical circulating cells stained by CD61, CD16/CD11b or unstained.  Introduction of CD11b and CD16 into the CTCs exclusion panel reduced the level of unidentified cells of epithelial origin in HV samples from 70% to 50%, while no difference was observed in MBC samples. The use of CD61 further reduced the proportion of cells that were unidentified in HV subjects from 70% to 30%, while it did not affect positivity rate in MBC patients. CD61 was expressed by a large percentage of other non-typical blood cells, while no signal was observed in patients’ CTCs. Morphological evaluation further reduced unidentified epithelial events in healthy subjects to 15%. (D) Representative image of a CTC (top) and a non-typical circulating cell (bottom). Non-typical circulating cells have diameter of 20 – 80 µm and can be differentiated from CTCs based on CK signal distribution, cell size and nuclear/cytoplasmic ratio. Typically, in CTCs, the CK signal is localized in the cytoskeleton, in a ring-like pattern. CTCs are also characterized by misshapen nucleus, which appears brighter and more condensed compared to leukocytes’ nuclei. Non-typical blood cells have a large nucleus (>20 µm in diameter) with no/limited cytoplasm and present low CK expression localized as a diffused signal in the nuclear area with an overlap between DAPI and CK signals. Slides were imaged using a 10× objective on BioView Allegro Plus system and are shown with 4× post imaging zoom. Merge colors: CD61 (white), DAPI (blue), CK8/CK18/CK19 (green), EpCAM (red). Micron bar = 30 µm.

13046_2024_3149_MOESM5_ESM.pdf

Additional File 5. Up to 15 contrived harvest sample slides containing WBCs and SKBR3 cells were stained with the final optimized panel. MFI (normalized for imaging exposure) of CK8/CK18/CK19 in AF488, EpCAM in AF555 and CD45/CD16/CD11b/CD61 in APC was assessed in positive and negative control cells to assess assay analytical sensitivity and specificity, respectively. (A) Histogram shows mean ± SEM of the MFI of each target in positive and negative cells. Green, orange and red lines show MFI values indicating detectable signal for CKs, EpCAM and CD45/CD16/CD11b/CD61, respectively. Mann-Whitney test was applied for significance between positive and negative cells, ****p≤0.0001. Analytical specificity and analytical sensitivity of CKs and CD markers was higher than 98%. EpCAM expression in SKBR3 and WBCs varied with a positivity rate of 80% and 6%, respectively. (B) Table shows number and percentage of SKBR3 cells (top row) and WBCs (bottom row) with detectable signal for CKs, EpCAM or CD markers.

13046_2024_3149_MOESM6_ESM.pdf

Additional File 6. CellTracker TM Orange prelabelled SKBR3 cells were spiked into K 2 EDTA tubes from 16 healthy volunteer subjects and separated through Parsortix ® instruments within 8 hours from draw. Samples were harvested into cytoslides and stained. Histogram shows mean ± SEM of the percentage of CellTracker™ Orange SKBR3 cells found in slide before and after staining compared to the number of cells captured in Parsortix ® separation cassette. Approximately 4% cell loss was observed following staining. Paired t-test applied; no statistically significant difference observed. The harvest processed combined with depositing cells onto cytoslides caused a mean cell loss of 62.3%.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Ciccioli, M., Kim, K., Khazan , N. et al. Identification of circulating tumor cells captured by the FDA-cleared Parsortix ® PC1 system from the peripheral blood of metastatic breast cancer patients using immunofluorescence and cytopathological evaluations. J Exp Clin Cancer Res 43 , 240 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13046-024-03149-x

Download citation

Received : 03 May 2024

Accepted : 03 August 2024

Published : 21 August 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s13046-024-03149-x

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Parsortix ® PC1 system
  • Liquid biopsy

Journal of Experimental & Clinical Cancer Research

ISSN: 1756-9966

  • Submission enquiries: Access here and click Contact Us
  • General enquiries: [email protected]

example of research experimental

example of research experimental

Extract insights from Customer & Employee Interviews. At Scale.

Experimental vs. control group explained.

Insight7

Home » Experimental vs. Control Group Explained

Group Comparison Analysis plays a pivotal role in experimental research. By examining the differences between experimental and control groups, researchers can draw meaningful conclusions about specific interventions. This process helps in determining whether observed effects are indeed attributable to the treatment or merely due to chance.

In any experiment, understanding how participants respond to different conditions is crucial. Group Comparison Analysis allows scientists to tease apart these responses, yielding insights that can inform various fields. Ultimately, this analytical approach not only enhances the validity of research findings but also supports the development of effective strategies based on empirical evidence.

The Basics of Experimental Groups

In research, understanding the distinction between experimental groups is essential for accurate findings. An experimental group consists of participants exposed to a variable being tested, while a control group serves as the baseline for comparison. This design enhances the reliability of results by isolating the effects of the independent variable. To conduct a thorough group comparison analysis, researchers need to ensure that both groups are similar in characteristics, minimizing biases.

The selection of participants plays a crucial role in the integrity of the study. Random assignment helps to ensure that individuals in both groups do not display pre-existing differences. This allows researchers to draw valid conclusions regarding the impact of the experimental treatment. Analyzing data from both groups provides insights into whether the intervention produces the expected changes. Effective comparison between these groups is foundational for advancing scientific knowledge. Understanding these basics will guide you through interpreting research outcomes with confidence.

Definition and Purpose

Understanding the experimental and control groups is essential in any Group Comparison Analysis. The experimental group receives the treatment or intervention, while the control group serves as a baseline for comparison. This structure is pivotal in determining the effectiveness of a given treatment and minimizes bias, ensuring the results are reliable.

The purpose of utilizing these groups lies in establishing a clear cause-and-effect relationship. By comparing outcomes from both groups, researchers can identify any significant differences attributable to the treatment. This comparison not only enhances the validity of findings but also influences data-driven decisions in various fields, including healthcare and marketing. Ultimately, the insight gained from this method fosters informed strategies that can lead to improved outcomes, whether in product development or user experience.

Designing an Experimental Group: Group Comparison Analysis

Designing an experimental group involves carefully planning each aspect to ensure valid results through group comparison analysis. This analysis is crucial for distinguishing the effects of a treatment or intervention from the natural variability found in any population. To effectively design your experimental group, you need to determine the characteristics that will make it comparable to the control group.

A proper comparison requires selection criteria such as age, gender, and baseline characteristics. This helps ensure that differences in outcomes arise solely from the intervention rather than from pre-existing variances. Next, consider randomization; randomly assigning participants reduces bias and enhances the study's reliability. Lastly, maintaining consistency in treatment delivery is essential. This ensures that everyone in the experimental group receives the same intervention, thus allowing for an accurate analysis of effects. By following these principles, your group comparison analysis can yield insightful and actionable outcomes.

The Role of Control Groups in Research

Control groups play a vital role in research by providing a benchmark to which experimental groups can be compared. Through group comparison analysis, researchers can discern the effects of an intervention by measuring outcomes against the control group that does not receive the treatment. This approach ensures that any observed changes in the experimental group can be more confidently attributed to the treatment rather than other external factors.

Moreover, control groups help minimize bias and variability in research outcomes. By allowing researchers to assess how participants behave under standard conditions, it becomes easier to isolate the impact of the experimental variable. Understanding these dynamics improves the reliability of results, making findings more valid and generalizable. Therefore, incorporating control groups in studies is essential for achieving accurate and trustworthy conclusions that can inform future practices or theories.

Definition and Purpose of Control Groups in Group Comparison Analysis

Control groups are essential in group comparison analysis, serving as benchmarks for experimental outcomes. These groups consist of participants who do not receive the treatment or intervention under investigation, allowing researchers to isolate the impact of specific variables. By comparing the results from the experimental group against the control group, researchers can determine the effectiveness of the intervention in a more precise manner.

The purpose of control groups is to minimize biases and ensure valid conclusions. They help in identifying whether observed changes in the experimental group are genuinely caused by the treatment or merely due to external factors. Additionally, control groups enable replication of studies, which is vital for affirming findings and fostering scientific credibility. In summary, control groups are indispensable tools in group comparison analysis, providing clarity and enhancing the reliability of research outcomes.

Examples of Control Group Usage

Control groups are essential in various fields, enabling researchers to validate their findings by providing a baseline for comparison. For instance, in a clinical trial assessing a new medication, one group receives the drug while a control group receives a placebo. This setup allows for a clearer understanding of the drug's effectiveness versus no treatment at all.

In market research, control groups allow analysts to examine consumer behavior under different conditions. A common example is testing two marketing strategies: one group receives traditional ads, while the control group is exposed to digital campaigns. Group comparison analysis reveals which method resonates better with the audience, helping to refine marketing approaches and optimize future campaigns. Through these examples, it's evident that control groups are invaluable in ensuring scientific rigor and making informed decisions across various domains.

Conclusion: The Importance of Group Comparison Analysis in Research

Group Comparison Analysis serves as a critical tool for researchers, allowing them to discern the differences between experimental and control groups. By methodically comparing these groups, researchers can assess the effectiveness of interventions or treatments. This type of analysis provides vital insights, facilitating a deeper understanding of how variables impact outcomes.

Furthermore, the importance of this analysis extends beyond mere statistical significance. It fosters evidence-based decision-making, ensuring that findings are reliable and applicable in real-world settings. Ultimately, understanding the dynamics between different groups equips researchers with the knowledge to make informed conclusions, driving advancements in various fields of study.

Turn interviews into actionable insights

On this Page

Random Sampling Definition in Research

You may also like, ai ml use cases for business innovation.

Insight7

Top Companies Using AI in 2024

Ai computer program solutions for businesses.

Unlock Insights from Interviews 10x faster

example of research experimental

  • Request demo
  • Get started for free

IMAGES

  1. 10 Real-Life Experimental Research Examples (2024)

    example of research experimental

  2. Experimental Research Methodology Examples : Research Methods

    example of research experimental

  3. What is Experimental Research & How is it Significant for Your Business

    example of research experimental

  4. Experimental Research

    example of research experimental

  5. Experimental research

    example of research experimental

  6. Experimental Research

    example of research experimental

COMMENTS

  1. 10 Real-Life Experimental Research Examples (2024)

    Examples of Experimental Research. 1. Pavlov's Dog: Classical Conditioning. Pavlovs Dogs. Dr. Ivan Pavlov was a physiologist studying animal digestive systems in the 1890s. In one study, he presented food to a dog and then collected its salivatory juices via a tube attached to the inside of the animal's mouth.

  2. 19+ Experimental Design Examples (Methods + Types)

    A famous example is the research conducted to test the effectiveness of different public health interventions, like vaccination programs. ... In our metaphorical research team, if True Experimental Design is the quarterback, Longitudinal Design is the wise elder, Factorial Design is the strategist, Cross-Sectional Design is the speedster ...

  3. Experimental Research Designs: Types, Examples & Advantages

    Experimental research design is a framework of protocols and procedures created to conduct experimental research with a scientific approach using two sets of variables. Herein, the first set of variables acts as a constant, used to measure the differences of the second set. The best example of experimental research methods is quantitative research.

  4. Experimental Research Designs: Types, Examples & Methods

    The pre-experimental research design is further divided into three types. One-shot Case Study Research Design. In this type of experimental study, only one dependent group or variable is considered. The study is carried out after some treatment which was presumed to cause change, making it a posttest study.

  5. Experimental Design: Types, Examples & Methods

    Three types of experimental designs are commonly used: 1. Independent Measures. Independent measures design, also known as between-groups, is an experimental design where different participants are used in each condition of the independent variable. This means that each condition of the experiment includes a different group of participants.

  6. A Quick Guide to Experimental Design

    Step 1: Define your variables. You should begin with a specific research question. We will work with two research question examples, one from health sciences and one from ecology: Example question 1: Phone use and sleep. You want to know how phone use before bedtime affects sleep patterns.

  7. Exploring Experimental Research: Methodologies, Designs, and

    Experimental research serves as a fundamental scientific method aimed at unraveling. cause-and-effect relationships between variables across various disciplines. This. paper delineates the key ...

  8. Experimental Design

    Examples of Experimental Design . Here are some examples of experimental design in different fields: Example in Medical research: A study that investigates the effectiveness of a new drug treatment for a particular condition. Patients are randomly assigned to either a treatment group or a control group, with the treatment group receiving the ...

  9. Experimental Research: Definition, Types and Examples

    The three main types of experimental research design are: 1. Pre-experimental research. A pre-experimental research study is an observational approach to performing an experiment. It's the most basic style of experimental research. Free experimental research can occur in one of these design structures: One-shot case study research design: In ...

  10. Experimental Research: What it is + Types of designs

    Experimental research is a study conducted with a scientific approach using two sets of variables. The first set acts as a constant, which you use to measure the differences of the second set. Quantitative research methods, for example, are experimental. If you don't have enough data to support your decisions, you must first determine the ...

  11. 15 Experimental Design Examples (2024)

    Experimental design involves testing an independent variable against a dependent variable. It is a central feature of the scientific method.. A simple example of an experimental design is a clinical trial, where research participants are placed into control and treatment groups in order to determine the degree to which an intervention in the treatment group is effective.

  12. Experimental Research Design

    Experimental research design is centrally concerned with constructing research that is high in causal (internal) validity. Randomized experimental designs provide the highest levels of causal validity. Quasi-experimental designs have a number of potential threats to their causal validity. Yet, new quasi-experimental designs adopted from fields ...

  13. Guide to experimental research design

    Experimental design is a research method that enables researchers to assess the effect of multiple factors on an outcome.. You can determine the relationship between each of the variables by: Manipulating one or more independent variables (i.e., stimuli or treatments). Applying the changes to one or more dependent variables (i.e., test groups or outcomes)

  14. Research Design

    Table of contents. Step 1: Consider your aims and approach. Step 2: Choose a type of research design. Step 3: Identify your population and sampling method. Step 4: Choose your data collection methods. Step 5: Plan your data collection procedures. Step 6: Decide on your data analysis strategies.

  15. Experimental Design

    Experimental Design | Types, Definition & Examples. Published on June 9, 2024 by Julia Merkus, MA.Revised on July 22, 2024. An experimental design is a systematic plan for conducting an experiment that aims to test a hypothesis or answer a research question.. It involves manipulating one or more independent variables (IVs) and measuring their effect on one or more dependent variables (DVs ...

  16. Experimental Research: Definition, Types, Examples

    Content. Experimental research is a cornerstone of scientific inquiry, providing a systematic approach to understanding cause-and-effect relationships and advancing knowledge in various fields. At its core, experimental research involves manipulating variables, observing outcomes, and drawing conclusions based on empirical evidence.

  17. What is experimental research: Definition, types & examples

    An example of experimental research in marketing: The ideal goal of a marketing product, advertisement, or campaign is to attract attention and create positive emotions in the target audience. Marketers can focus on different elements in different campaigns, change the packaging/outline, and have a different approach.

  18. Experimental Research

    Experimental research is commonly used in sciences such as sociology and psychology, physics, chemistry, biology and medicine etc. It is a collection of research designs which use manipulation and controlled testing to understand causal processes. Generally, one or more variables are manipulated to determine their effect on a dependent variable.

  19. Experimental Research

    Experimental science is the queen of sciences and the goal of all speculation. Roger Bacon (1214-1294) Download chapter PDF. Experiments are part of the scientific method that helps to decide the fate of two or more competing hypotheses or explanations on a phenomenon. The term 'experiment' arises from Latin, Experiri, which means, 'to ...

  20. Experimental Research

    In true experimental research, the researcher not only manipulates the independent variable, he or she also randomly assigned individuals to the various treatment categories ... For example, a researcher could be studying the effect of laptop computers on students' attitudes toward math. The teacher feels sorry for the class that doesn't ...

  21. Experimental Research: Meaning And Examples Of Experimental ...

    Experimental research is widely implemented in education, psychology, social sciences and physical sciences. Experimental research is based on observation, calculation, comparison and logic. Researchers collect quantitative data and perform statistical analyses of two sets of variables. This method collects necessary data to focus on facts and ...

  22. Experimental Research Designs: Definition, Types and Examples

    1) Experimental research focuses on understanding the nature of relationship between independent and dependent variables involved under a particular field of study. On the other hand , Non-experimental research is descriptive in nature and so , focuses on defining a process , situation or idea. 2) Experimental research provides the freedom to ...

  23. 10 Research Question Examples to Guide your Research Project

    The first question asks for a ready-made solution, and is not focused or researchable. The second question is a clearer comparative question, but note that it may not be practically feasible. For a smaller research project or thesis, it could be narrowed down further to focus on the effectiveness of drunk driving laws in just one or two countries.

  24. Identification of circulating tumor cells captured by the FDA-cleared

    Circulating Tumor Cells (CTCs) may serve as a non-invasive source of tumor material to investigate an individual's disease in real-time. The Parsortix® PC1 System, the first FDA-cleared medical device for the capture and harvest of CTCs from peripheral blood of metastatic breast cancer (MBC) patients for use in subsequent user-validated downstream analyses, enables the epitope-independent ...

  25. Experimental vs. Control Group Explained

    An experimental group consists of participants exposed to a variable being tested, while a control group serves as the baseline for comparison. This design enhances the reliability of results by isolating the effects of the independent variable. ... providing clarity and enhancing the reliability of research outcomes. Examples of Control Group ...

  26. Development and Insulation Performance Evaluation of Experimental

    The insulation performances of the experimental sample at different SF 6 gas pressures are investigated, and the leakage current laws of the experimental sample at different applied voltages and different SF 6 gas pressures are obtained. The test results show that the maximum leakage current is 472 μA at a loading voltage of 500 kV, which ...